Download PDF
ads:
UNIVERSIDADE FEDERAL FLUMINENSE
CENTRO DE ESTUDOS GERAIS
INSTITUTO DE LETRAS
COORDENAÇÃO DE PÓS-GRADUAÇÃO EM LÍNGÜÍSTICA APLICADA
CRISTIANE DA SILVA LOPES
A CONTRASTIVE GENRE-ORIENTED INVESTIGATION OF READING
COMPREHENSION SECTIONS FROM EFL PROFICIENCY TESTS AND EFL
COURSEBOOKS
Niterói – RJ
2006
ads:
Livros Grátis
http://www.livrosgratis.com.br
Milhares de livros grátis para download.
CRISTIANE DA SILVA LOPES
A CONTRASTIVE GENRE-ORIENTED INVESTIGATION OF READING
COMPREHENSION SECTIONS FROM EFL PROFICIENCY TESTS AND EFL
COURSEBOOKS
Dissertação apresentada ao Programa de
Pós-graduação em Letras da Universidade
Federal Fluminense como requisito parcial
para a obtenção do título de Mestre em
Lingüística Aplicada ao Ensino de Língua
Estrangeira. Orientador: Prof. Dr. David
Shepherd
Niterói – RJ
2006
ads:
L864 Lopes, Cristiane da Silva.
A contrastive genre-oriented investigation of reading
comprehension sections from EFL Proficiency Tests and EFL
Coursebooks / Cristiane da Silva Lopes. – 2006.
156 f.
Orientador: David Shepherd.
Dissertação (Mestrado) – Universidade Federal Fluminense,
Instituto de Letras, 2006.
Bibliografia: f. 151-156.
1. Língua inglesa – Estudo e ensino. 2. Língua inglesa -
Gênero. 3. Leitura. 4. Testes. I. Shepherd, David. II.
Universidade Federal Fluminense. Instituto de Letras. III. Título.
CDD 420.7
CRISTIANE DA SILVA LOPES
A CONTRASTIVE GENRE-ORIENTED INVESTIGATION OF READING
COMPREHENSION SECTIONS FROM EFL PROFICIENCY TESTS AND EFL
COURSEBOOKS
Dissertação apresentada ao Programa de
Pós-graduação em Letras da Universidade
Federal Fluminense como requisito parcial
para a obtenção do título de Mestre em
Lingüística Aplicada ao Ensino de Língua
Estrangeira.
BANCA EXAMINADORA
____________________________________________
Prof. Dr. David Shepherd (Orientador)
Universidade Federal Fluminense
____________________________________________
Profa. Dra. Solange Coelho Vereza
Universidade Federal Fluminense
____________________________________________
Profa. Dra. Gisele de Carvalho
Universidade Estadual do Rio de Janeiro
To the memory of my grandmother, Maria
do Carmo.
Iii
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
First and foremost, to God for providing me with guidance and support
throughout the most difficult moments of my existence
To my supervisor, Prof. Dr. David Shepherd, for his patience and
encouragement. Without his valuable support and dedication, the painstaking
task of developing the present research would not have been possible.
To Profa. Dra. Maria Elisa Knust and Profa. Dra. Sonia Torres, who have
enormously contributed to my academic growth.
To Profa. Dra. Solange Coelho Vereza, who always believed in my potential
and encouraged me to carry on in the two most difficult moments of my life.
To my immediate family for their love and support.
To my mother, Sebastiana, who never let me give up despite the difficulties.
To my dear husband João Carlos for reading drafts of this dissertation and for
the kind words of emotional support.
To my friends Cláudia, Elen Lessa, Nelma Pedretti, Roseli Ferreira and
Verônica de Fátima for their supportive friendship.
Iv
RESUMO
O principal objetivo da presente pesquisa consiste em desenvolver uma análise
contrastiva das seções de leitura de livros didáticos para o ensino de inglês
como língua estrangeira e das provas que testam as habilidades de leitura em
exames de proficiência. Tal investigação é desenvolvida sob a perspectiva dos
estudos empreendidos no campo da análise de gêneros textuais. O corpus é
constituído por quatro provas de leitura que compõem os exames FCE dos
anos 2003 e 2004 e por vinte seções destinadas ao desenvolvimento das
habilidades de leitura em cinco livros didáticos amplamente utilizados em
instituições privadas para o ensino de línguas no Brasil. Apesar das inúmeras
investigações desenvolvidas no campo dos gêneros textuais e da gramática
funcional sistêmica, observou-se nos elementos do corpus um caráter
incidental no que diz respeito ao emprego de atividades destinadas à
percepção de aspectos organizacionais reveladores do propósito comunicativo
do gênero ao qual o texto alvo pertence. Tal constatação parece sugerir que a
contemplação de aspectos inerentes ao gênero textual ainda não constitui uma
prática na sala de aula de leitura em língua estrangeira. A partir da análise
empreendida foi possível concluir que o potencial pedagógico dos estudos
empreendidos na área dos gêneros textuais ainda não resultou em mudanças
significativas para a abordagem dos textos dentro da sala de aula de leitura em
língua estrangeira.
Palavras-chaves: gênero – leitura – testagem
v
ABSTRACT
The ultimate aim of the present research is to develop a genre-oriented
investigation of the reading comprehension sections from EFL proficiency tests
in contrast to EFL coursebooks. The investigation opted for the analysis of four
FCE reading papers of the years 2003 and 2004 plus twenty reading sections
from five distinct EFL coursebooks widely used in Brazilian private language
schools. Despite the advances in the area of genre studies, the analysis of the
elements of the corpus reveals an incidental nature as far as the genre-focused
questions are concerned. The identification of generic features such as aspects
of field, tenor and mode does not constitute a common practice. Moreover, the
questions focused on the schematic organization of target texts do not seem to
overtly contribute to the identification of the communicative purposes of the
texts to which they are related. These findings made it possible to conclude that
the potential pedagogical applications of the investigations in the area of genre
studies do not seem to be acknowledged as a crucial element in order to
develop learners` proficiency in reading in the target language.
Key words: genre – reading – testing
Vi
LIST OF ABBREVIATIONS
ALTE
Association of Language Testers in Europe
CAE
Certificate in Advanced English. Universidade de Cambridge (UCLES)
CCAA
Centro de Cultura Anglo-Americana
CPE
Certificate of Proficiency in English. Universidade de Cambridge (UCLES)
EAP
English for Academic Purposes
EFL
English as a foreign language
ESP
English for Specific Purposes
FCE
First Certificate in English. Universidade de Cambridge (UCLES)
FL
Foreign language
GMAT
Graduate Management Admissions Test. Graduate Management
Admission Council.
IELTS
International English Language Testing System (UCLES)
KET
Key English Test. Universidade de Cambridge (UCLES)
L1
First language
L2
Second language
PET
Preliminary English Test. Universidade de Cambridge (UCLES)
SBCI
Sociedade Brasileira de Cultura Inglesa
TEFL
Teachers of English as a foreign language
TKT
Teacher Knowledge Test
TOEFL
Test of Overseas English as a Foreign Language. Educational Testing
Service (New Jersey)
UCLES
University of Cambridge Local Examinations Syndicate
vii
LIST OF FIGURES
Figure 1: Genre-texts presented in the format of articles ……………..…….. 110
viii
LIST OF GRAPHS
Graph 1: Occurrences of genre-texts in the coursebooks ……………..……… 96
Graph 2: Predominant genre-texts in coursebook “Alfa”………….…….……..108
Graph 3: Predominant genre-texts in coursebook “Beta” ……………………. 108
Graph 4: Predominant genre-texts in coursebook “Gama” ………………..… 108
Graph 5: Predominant genre-texts in coursebook “Delta” …………………... 109
Graph 6: Predominant genre-texts in coursebook “Épsilon” …………..……. 109
Graph 7: Predominant non-genre oriented questions in the coursebooks …. 128
investigated.
Graph 8: Predominant genre-oriented questions in the coursebooks ………. 131
investigated.
Graph 9: Predominant non-genre oriented questions in the FCE
Reading Papers …………………………………………………..……. 134
Graph 10: Predominant genre-oriented questions in the FCE
Reading Papers …………………………………………………….... 134
ix
LIST OF TABLES
Table 2.1.: Cambridge EFL examinations ………………………………………. 45
Table 2.2.: Bhatia’s Promotional Genre Framework for analyzing
sales promotion letters ……………………………….………………. 67
Table 2.3.: Bhatia’s Promotional Genre Framework for analyzing job
application letters ……………………………………..………………. 68
Table 2.4.: Motta-Roth’s (1995) academic book review moves ………………. 68
Table 2.5.: Swales’ CARS model for article introductions ………………..……. 69
Table 2.6.: Genre-general patterns ………………………………………….…… 70
Table 3.1.: Samples of specific acronyms …………………
Table 3.2..: Samples of specific lexis ………………………..
Table 3.3..: Genre-texts identified in the coursebooks ……………….
Table 3.4..: The layout features identified in Text ONE …………………..…… 98
Table 3.5..: The identification of macro-patterns via
question-and-answer dialog ………………………………..……… 101
Table 3.6..: Samples of lexico-grammatical choices in Text ONE ……….….. 102
Table 3.7..: The layout features identified in Text TWO …………………….... 103
Table 3.8..: The identification of macro-patterns via
question-and-answer dialog …………………………………….... 104
Table 3.9..: Samples of lexico-grammatical choices in Text TWO ………….. 105
X
Table 3.10.: Salient genre-texts identified in the FCE
Reading Paper/June2003 …………………………….………….. 112
Table 3.11.: Salient genre-texts identified in the FCE
Reading Paper/Dec.2003 ………………………….……………… 112
Table 3.12.: Salient genre-texts identified in the FCE
Reading Paper/June2004 ……………………………….………… 112
Table 3.13.: Salient genre-texts identified in the FCE
Reading Paper/Dec2004 ………………………………………….. 113
Table 3.14.: An overview of the comprehension questions
identified in the coursebooks …………………………………….. 127
Table 3.15.: An overview of the comprehension questions
identified in the FCE reading papers …………………………….. 133
xi
TABLE OF CONTENTS
List of Abbreviations …………………………………………………………… vii
List of Figures ……………………………………………………………….….. viii
List of Graphs …………………………………………………………………….. ix
List of Tables ………………………………..........................................………. x
CHAPTER 1: INTRODUCTION
1.1. Situating the problem: EFL reading lessons vs global market: a
contradictory scenario ......................................................................... 01
1.2. Objectives: outlining research phases ................................................ 06
CHAPTER 2: REVIEW OF THE RELEVANT LITERATURE
2.1. Approaching EFL reading ................................................................ 08
2.1.1. Defining reading …………………………………………………............ 08
2.1.2. Views on reading ……………………………….................................... 10
2.1.2.1. The cognitive processing of texts ……………………………………… 11
2.1.2.2. A schema-oriented view of reading ……………………..………….… 13
2.1.3. Reading models: three distinct approaches to reading ………….….. 17
2.1.4. Reading skills and strategies ….……………………………………..… 20
2.1.5. The three major constituents of the reading process ....................... 24
2.1.6. Proficient readers: a combination of reading skills and strategies …26
2.1.7. The relevance of EFL reading within a global scenario .................... 30
2.1.8. Approaching reading within overseas settings .................................. 32
2.1.9. An enlightened approach to EFL reading via “alien” coursebooks … 37
2.2. The testing of reading …………………………………………….…… 42
2.2.1. Introduction ……………………………………………………..……… 42
2.2.2. Background to the FCE …………………………..…………………… 43
2.2.2.1. General considerations regarding the UCLES ……………………... 43
Xii
2.2.2.2. The FCE ………………………………………………………………… 44
2.2.3. Similarities between teaching and testing reading ……………….… 46
2.2.4. Differences between teaching and testing reading ………………… 47
2.2.5. The relevance of Washback and Impact Research ………….…...…51
2.2.6. Learning validity: a concern in language testing ……………….…...53
2.3. Revisiting Genre Theories ……………………………………….……55
2.3.1. Background to Text Linguistics ……………………………..………. 55
2.3.2. Approaching reading via text analysis …………………….…………60
2.3.2.1. The interactive nature of reading …………………………..…………60
2.3.2.2. Texts are cohesive …………………………………………..…………63
2.3.2.3. Texts are patterned .........................................................................65
2.3.3. The genre-specific patterns ............................................................67
2.3.4. The genre-general patterns ……………………………….…………..70
2.3.5. Defining genre: Swales’ (1990) Working Definition of Genre ……. 72
2.3.6. The social roles of genres ……………………………………………..74
2.3.7. The Context of Culture ………………………………………………...78
2.3.8. The Context of Situation ………………………….……………………78
2.4. Conclusions ……………………………………………………………80
CHAPTER 3: METHODOLOGY
3.1. Selecting the corpus …………………………………………..……….83
3.2. Describing the corpus ………………………………………………….84
3.2.1. The coursebooks ……………………………………………………... 84
3.2.1.1. The Coursebook “Alfa” ………………………………….………….... 85
3.2.1.2. The Coursebook “Beta” ………………………………….…………….85
3.2.1.3. The Coursebook “Gama” ……………………………………..……… 86
3.2.1.4. The Coursebook “Delta” ……………………………………………... 87
3.2.1.5. The Coursebook “Épsilon” …………………………………………….87
3.2.2. The FCE Reading Papers ……………………………………………..88
3.3. Collecting and analyzing data ………………………………………..88
3.3.1. Introduction …………………………………………………………... 88
3.3.2. The Discourse Community behind the FCE examination ………….90
xiii
3.3.3. A quantitative-interpretative analysis of the genre-texts identified ..95
3.3.3.1. In the coursebooks …………………………………………………..…95
3.3.3.1.1. Sample Text ONE …………………………………………………..…97
3.3.3.1.2. Sample Text TWO ……………………………..…………………….102
3.3.4. Predominant genre-texts …………………………………………… 107
3.3.5. Preliminary conclusions ………………………………………….… 109
3.3.6. Predominant genre-texts in the FCE reading paper ……………. 112
3.3.7. Preliminary conclusions ………………………………………….… 114
3.3.8. Analysis of the reading comprehension questions ……………… 115
3.3.8.1. Introduction ………………………………………………..………… 115
3.3.8.2. Analyzing data ……………………………………………….……… 117
3.3.8.2.1. The non-genre oriented questions ………………………………… 117
3.3.8.2.2. The genre-oriented questions ……………………………………… 122
3.3.9. Main findings: contrasting data …………………….……………… 126
3.3.9.1. Introduction ……………………….............................…………….. 126
3.3.9.2. The Coursebooks ………………………………………………….. 127
3.3.9.3. The FCE reading papers …………………………………………... 133
CHAPTER 4: CONCLUSIONS
4.1. Final conclusions ……………………………..…………………….. 136
4.2. Potential pedagogical insights …………………….………………. 142
4.3. Limitations and further research ……………….………………….. 148
Bibliography ................................................................................................151
Appendices …………………………………………………………………..… 157
Appendix 1: Sample texts from the five coursebooks
Coursebook “Alfa”
Coursebook “Beta”
Coursebook “Gama”
Coursebook “Delta”
Coursebook “Épsilon”
Xiv
Appendix 2: The four FCE reading papers
June/2003
December/2003
June/2004
December/2004
Appendix 3: Table 3.1: Samples of specific acronyms
Appendix 4: Table 3.2: Samples of specific lexis
Appendix 5: Table 3.3: Genre-texts identified in the coursebooks
Appendix 6: Sample Text ONE: Do we really need to sleep?
Appendix 7: Sample Text TWO: Friends again forever.
Appendix 8: Sample Text: Biographical sketch
Appendix 9: Sample Text: Travel brochure
Appendix 10: Sample Text: an interview
xv
1
CHAPTER 1
1. INTRODUCTION
1.1. Situating the problem: EFL reading lessons versus global market
demands: a contradictory scenario.
My experience as an EFL teacher in a range of Brazilian private language schools
seems to suggest that, despite the development of research studies in the field of
reading, there still seems to be a tendency to regard the development of effective
reading skills as secondary. Speaking, listening, in first place, and also writing
skills have long constituted the major concern within EFL classrooms. This
situation may be said to emerge from a diversity of factors such as tight schedules,
lack of expertise on the teachers’ part and learners’ apparent lack of interest in
reading. This scenario generated the reflections outlined below. Such reflections,
by the same token, led to the development of the research study presented in the
subsequent chapters.
Investigations developed in the area of EFL teaching/learning have revealed a
global tendency to acknowledge the mastery of the English language as an
indispensable tool in the growing competitive global scenario. English is claimed to
have reached the status of a global language (Crystal, 1997:2) and is often
regarded as the major tool for international communication in different parts of the
globe. Recent statistics (Graddol, 2006:62) have revealed that the number of L2
and FL speakers of English is steadily growing. Furthermore, the English language
has long been regarded as a vehicle in international transactions. As Crystal
(1997:2) states, The English language is spoken by politicians from all over the world
2
(...) wherever you travel, you see English signs and advertisements. Graddol (1999:57)
subscribes to Crystal’s (op. cit.) views and contrasts the sharp growth in the
number of L2 speakers of English, compared to the number of L1 speakers, which
appears to be declining. According to this latter author, such a phenomenon may
be explained from three distinct perspectives. Firstly, there seems to exist
demographic evidence which suggests that the proportion of the world population
which uses English as L1 has been steadily decreasing. Secondly, English has
achieved an international status, which reveals this language as the main tool to
be used in multilingual contexts
1
. Thirdly, Graddol (1999:57) avers that the
phenomenon previously described might be related to changes in the conception
of languages, linguistic competence and identity. However, more recently,
Graddol’s (2006:62) investigations seem to indicate that nowadays the status of
the English language is being challenged. According to Graddol (2006:62) English
is by no means the only language in global business
2
. However, considering that the
main aim of this section is to provide no more than a picture of the relevance of the
English language within the global scenario, the main causes for the rise in the
number of speakers of other languages will not be addressed.
In 1995, Crystal (1997, apud Graddol, 1999:62) conducted a detailed investigation
on the use of the English language in 66 countries. At that time, his findings
1
Within these contexts, English is regarded as the vehicle which allows communication among
non-native speakers to occur.
2
It seems important to highlight that more recent investigations have signaled that English can no
longer be regarded as the only language in global business transactions (David, 2003 apud Graddol,
2006:62). According to Graddol (2006:62), the dominance of the English language is being
challenged in some parts of the globe by other languages such as Mandarin and Spanish, which
have reached higher status and, as a consequence, have influenced national policy priorities in
certain nations.
3
revealed that there were about 235 millions speakers of English in the world. In
addition, the same author argued that the number of L2 speakers of English is
expected to reach 462 millions by the year 2045. These figures seem to suggest a
substantial change: the growing number of L2 English speakers which is likely to
outnumber the number of L1 speakers. Similarly, the continuous globalization
phenomenon and the growing urge for a language of international communication
has placed the English language as the language of business transactions into the
international arena. Thus, in summary, it is possible to argue that learning to read
in English has become an indispensable ingredient nowadays, particularly for
professionals who wish to improve their opportunities in the competitive job market
or for those professionals, in all spheres of activity, including academia, whose
progress depends on accessing up-to-date knowledge regarding research.
In Brazil, this need has led to a growing interest for the learning of the English
language. As a consequence, a wide range of private language schools, often
called “cursinhos de inglês” have flourished. These institutions attempt to attract a
demanding and numerous clientele. As a result, the publishing market has also
flourished and become a profitable business due to the continuous demand for
coursebooks, among other pedagogical resources, for the teaching of the target
language.
Moreover, proficiency tests such as Michigan, TOEFL and the various Cambridge
examinations have extended their markets within this scenario, as they are seen
as a means to award or achieve internationally recognized certification for those
learners who have dedicated years to their language learning. These certificates
4
are said to provide their holders with a number of benefits, particularly in
professional settings. They are claimed to provide evidence that applicants are
able to communicate fluently using certain skills in the target language, which, in
turn, enhances the individual’s chances of obtaining an improved position or
gaining a promotion. In very general terms, most of these examinations are said to
test the mastery of the four skills, i.e., reading, writing, listening comprehension
and speaking. Moreover, they seem to follow a set of pre-established criteria
which must be fulfilled by candidates. Therefore, apart from being linguistically
competent, candidates are expected to take exam practice courses in order to
achieve positive results.
In an attempt to cope with the demands of the proficiency tests market, specific
training is provided by private language schools. More recently, with the advent of
the internet, a number of websites offer online training. Among the major
certificates of proficiency in the English language, it is worth highlighting
MICHIGAN, GMAT and the various Cambridge University qualifications such as
CPE, CAE, FCE, IELTS and TKT. It seems important to point out that substantial
compulsory fees must be paid by candidates prior to undergoing the exams.
As highlighted in the preceding paragraphs, most of these proficiency tests are
said to have as their ultimate aim an assessment of the mastery of the four skills.
The focal point of the present investigation however, has been limited to the
testing of reading. Through recent contact with the studies developed in the area
of systemic functional grammar and genre analysis, I have come to conclude that
there seems to exist aspects which are inherent in the teaching/learning of foreign
5
language reading and which seem to contribute to the development of more
proficient readers in the target language. Similarly, my own experience as an EFL
learner seems to suggest that “reading lessons”, at least in private language
schools, rarely appear to reflect the actual demands of a globalized market. The
latter demands proficient readers who are expected to be able to experiment and
analyze texts as a semantic entity. In contrast, the development of reading skills is
often given a secondary role with limited focus on the reading sections assigned
as homework.
Coursebooks emerge as a fundamental resource within this scenario. However,
depending on the institution
3
, they will give little or no emphasis to the
teaching/learning of reading skills. My experience in the light of the theoretical
discussions developed throughout my postgraduate courses, has demonstrated
that, very often, teachers seem to be concerned about focusing on no more than
one of the many aspects which reading appears to comprise: reading as a
decodification of printed symbols (Nuttall, 1996:2). Therefore, one of the most
important aspects related to the teaching of reading skills is often neglected,
namely, the triangular relationship of writer-text-reader. This reflection has led to
the three pedagogical questions which constitute the focal point of this
investigation:
1) Do reading sections of coursebooks really reflect the demands of a globalized
society in which individuals are expected to experiment with texts as semantic
units rather than groups of isolated sentences?
3
In the last three decades, several private language franchise schools (e.g.: CCAA, BRASAS,
SBCI, Wizard, CNA etc.) have opted to produce their own coursebooks.
6
2) To what extent can genre analysis contribute to the development of more
effective reading lessons and, as a result, guide learners towards becoming more
proficient readers and, potentially, as a consequence, improved EFL writers?
3) How genre-oriented are reading comprehension questions in the coursebooks
and in the FCE reading papers which constitute the corpus of analysis?
In an attempt to find answers to these pedagogical concerns, the research to be
presented will have as its theoretical foundation three distinct areas of study,
namely the views on reading, the testing of reading skills and genre studies.
1.2. OBJECTIVES: OUTLINING RESEARCH PHASES
The bulk of the present investigation consists of an attempt to answer the three
research questions outlined in Section 1.1. In order to achieve this aim, a
discussion will be conducted by bearing in mind three distinct fields of study,
namely reading, testing of reading and genre studies (Chapter 2). It is hoped that
these theoretical discussions and their applications will provide insightful
contributions for the development of the analysis of the corpus (Chapter 3) and, as
a result, enlighten the conclusions drawn in Chapter 4.
Chapter 2 provides the theoretical background for the development of the analysis.
Section 2.1 discusses the issues surrounding the relevance of teaching effective
FL reading strategies within the modern global scenario. Here, a critical token on
7
the adoption of “alien” coursebooks within non-native settings is also provided.
Section 2.2 addresses the testing of reading. In this same section, background
information relating to the FCE is provided, in view of the fact that the FCE reading
paper constitutes an integrating element of the corpus of analysis. The relevance
of Washback and Impact research investigations are also discussed in the same
section. Finally, Section 2.3 revisits genre theories, the concept of discourse
community postulated by Swales (1990) and discusses the relevance of text
analysis for the teaching and testing of reading.
Chapter 3 is concerned with the stages of the analysis itself. It provides a detailed
description of the corpus, as well as the set of criteria considered for its selection.
The criteria adopted for the analysis are provided in this same section. In addition,
preliminary conclusions are drawn.
The main findings and conclusions are drawn in Chapter 4. Furthermore, in this
same chapter, the potential pedagogical applications, as well as limitations of the
research are addressed. Finally, possibilities for further research in the area of
genre studies are suggested.
8
CHAPTER 2
A REVIEW OF THE RELEVANT LITERATURE
2.1. APPROACHING EFL READING
2.1.1. Defining reading
Celce-Murcia & Olshstain (2000:121) assert that the vast literature and substantial
amount of studies developed in the field of L2/FL reading in the past two decades
suggest that the nature of the reading process has long constituted a source of
concern within the second/foreign language teaching world. Since the 1970s the
approaches to reading have undergone continuous processes of reflection and
substantial transformations (Edmundson, 2004:19). Among the existing diversity of
theories on reading, Huey’s is acknowledged as one of the precursors
(Edmundson, op. cit.). According to Edmundson, Huey defines the act of reading
as follows:
Ler consiste em uma série de subprocessos que passam dos perceptuais
(extrair padrões visuais) aos lingüísticos, cognitivos e motores, cuja
integração é afetada tanto por fatores internos (autonomia, atenção e
capacidade de memória) quanto por variáveis externas (construções
sintáticas, ambigüidade léxica e a natureza do material a ser lido)
4
.
(Edmundson, 2004:19)
Edmundson claims that in Huey’s (op. cit.) viewpoint, the essence of the act of
reading lies in the combination of what the author defines as lower and higher-level
processing mechanisms. The former is concerned with the visual input (i.e.: the
4
Reading consists of a number of sub-processes which move from perceptual to linguistic or
cognitive and physical. The integration of such sub-processes tends to be affected by both internal
factors (i.e.: autonomy, attention and memory capacity) and external factors (i.e.: syntactic
constructions, lexical ambiguity and the nature of target reading material. [my translation]
9
recognition and decodification of linguistic symbols which enable readers to move
from smaller to bigger units such as in the sequence word-sentence-paragraph-
text, whereas the latter is concerned with semantic relationships which are likely to
promote comprehension.
Since then, over a century, as investigations on processes experienced by readers
evolved, a diversity of theories on how reading is processed has been developed.
Empirical evidence has been of substantial value and has provided insightful
contributions towards a more comprehensive understanding of the rationale
underlying the act of reading. The citations below exemplify the several definitions
for the term reading, which have been generated from research in this field:
The process of getting linguistic information via print. (Widdowson,
1979 apud Alderson & Urquhart,1984: XXV)
Reading, according to this view, is an interactive process – as
conversation is – because both reader and writer depend on one another.
(Nuttall, 1996:11)
O processo de ler é complexo. Como em outras tarefas cognitivas, como
resolver problemas, trazer à mente uma informação necessária, aplicar
algum conhecimento a uma situação nova, o engajamento de muitos
fatores (percepção, atenção, memória) é essencial se queremos fazer
sentido do texto
5
. (Kleiman, 2001:13)
Given that the written text also constitutes an integrating element of our
investigation, an attempt will now be made to provide, in the section below, a
5
The reading process is complex. As in other cognitive tasks, such as solving a problem, recalling
an essential piece of information, applying any kind of previous knowledge to a new situation and
engaging in a variety of other factors (i.e.: perception, attention and memory) is essential if we
really wish to make sense of a text. [my translation]
10
theoretically-based discussion on the nature of the act of reading and what its
processes imply. It is hoped that this discussion may prove to be particularly
relevant in order to achieve an improved understanding of the approaches which
are, either conscious or unconsciously, adopted for the teaching of reading
skills/strategies in a number of private EFL schools in Brazil.
2.1.2. Views on reading
Historically, reading was often regarded as a passive activity (Nuttall, 1996:5). It
was long defined as a serial or linear process (Kleiman, 2001:17) whereby the
decoder (hereafter reader) would be restricted to the mere recognition and
decodification of linguistic symbols. Kleiman (1996, apud Edmundson, 2004:20)
rejects this view of reading since it suggests a lack of interaction between lower
and higher-level processing mechanisms i.e., it does not acknowledge the
relationships established between linguistic, syntactic and semantic
interpretations. Nevertheless, the same author acknowledges the crucial
importance of lower-level processing mechanisms in order to achieve
comprehension. A further view regards reading as an opportunity either to teach
pronunciation or to practice fluent speaking (Nuttall, 1996:2).
These two views of reading are undeniably still widely held and have their place in
the reading class. However, up-to-date research studies (Nuttall, 1996:2) have
strengthened Huey’s (op. cit.) views and revealed that the term reading appears to
embody a considerably more comprehensive meaning which may be summarized,
in very general terms, as the conveying of a message from the printed page. As a
result, more recent research investigations in this area have scrutinized the nature
11
of the act of reading from a diversity of angles. Accordingly, readers should no
longer be viewed as “empty minds” to be filled with the message and knowledge
encoded by writers but as fundamental constituents of a dialogic process (Wallace,
1992:44-45). According to Edmundson (2004:22), Huey’s attempt, in 1908, to
describe reading as a combination of lower and higher-level mechanisms, was of
crucial importance since it triggered the development of further investigations on
the nature of the act of reading. Such investigations focused on reading from
cognitive, psycholinguistic and sociointeractive perspectives, which are further
discussed in the present research.
2.1.2.1. The cognitive processing of texts
According to Kleiman (2002:31), the view of reading as an activity which may be
taught within the classroom boundaries is strongly based on well-defined models
which attempt to describe how information from texts is perceived, first by the
eyes, and later processed by the brain. These models reflect the “psycholinguistic-
cognitive” nature of reading (i.e.: from the recognition of printed symbols to the
storage of information in our brains) as well as incorporate its sociocultural
aspects.
The first psycholinguistic reading models emerged in the 1970s with the advent of
Psycholinguistics. The psycholinguist view of reading acknowledges readers as
thinking individuals who feel and act within a given sociocultural context
(Edmundson, 2004:22). The insights generated from the psycholinguistic view
praised both cognitive (i.e.: linguistic aptitude and cognitive abilities) and affective
12
factors (i.e.: attitude and motivation) which seem to be inherent in the act of
reading.
Ler é formar um sentido da linguagem escrita, é um processo
psicolingüístico, pois uma teoria da leitura deveria incluir as relações
entre o pensamento e a linguagem
6
. (Goodman, 1976 apud
Edumundson 2004:22)
As mentioned above, Kleiman (op. cit.) explains that the processing of texts has
the eyes as a starting point. Nevertheless, the eyes enable readers only to partially
reconstruct the writer’s encoded message (Kleiman, 2002:33), i.e., the eyes
enable the perception of the printed symbols (i.e.: graphic input) which are later
sent to functional memory where they are organized into meaningful units.
Functional memory, by the same token, is aided by what Kleiman (op. cit.) defines
as an intermediate memory which is responsible for activating the previous
knowledge required for the comprehension of the given text. This knowledge is
organized within our long-term memory, also regarded as semantic memory or deep
memory (Kleiman, 2002:32) which represents the higher-level functions which allow
the construction of meanings. Thus, from a cognitive perspective, reading is
considered a psycholinguistic guessing game (Goodman, 1976:126-135) whereby
readers formulate hypotheses and combine their linguistic and conceptual
knowledge as well as their experiences so as to construct meanings.
Understanding a text does not result from establishing sequential connections
among the events in the text. Readers undergo inference procedures in order to
make sense of what they read.
6
Reading consists in making sense of written language. It is a psycholinguistic process. Thus, a
given theory of reading should include the relations between thinking and language. [my
translation]
13
In the early stages of literacy, reading is usually viewed by the reader as a
painstaking process restricted to the mere decodification of printed symbols.
However, possessing a more comprehensive knowledge of the psychological and
cognitive features inherent in the reading process has proved to be of substantial
relevance. Teachers, by resorting to such knowledge, are likely to enhance their
awareness regarding the pedagogical practices which might promote effective
reading comprehension among their learners. Reading activities are not intended
to be form-focused but meaning-focused and enriched by the teacher’s comments
and questions. As Kleiman (2002:35) argues: The teacher must provide learners with
contexts to which they may resort as a means to promote comprehension at a variety of
levels of knowledge (e.g.: graphic, linguistic, pragmatic, social and cultural).
Furthermore, a comprehensive knowledge of the psycholinguistic-cognitive nature of
reading enables teachers to determine probable sources of difficulties when
processing texts, as well as strategies for coping with such difficulties.
2.1.2.2. A schema-oriented view of reading
Reading as an interactive process (Ruddell, 1976 apud Edmundson, 2004:22)
presupposes the active involvement of both writer (i.e., encoder) and reader (i.e.,
decoder). Similarly, a prerequisite for successful communication to occur is that
the reader and writer have certain things in common (Nuttall, 1996:6). Firstly, both
must share the same code, i.e., they must write and understand the same
language, which suggests that they must have a similar command of the
language. Secondly, a further essential requirement is that the writer and reader
share certain assumptions about the world and the way it works (Nuttall, 1996:6).
14
Although mismatches of some kind between the writer’s and reader’s
presuppositions are likely to occur, they may be minimized by a set of shared
assumptions between the participants of the reading process. The major
component for successful reading however may be explained through the schema
view of reading (Nuttall, 1996:7). Nuttall defines the schema theory as follows:
A schema (plural schemata) is a mental structure. It is abstract because
it does not relate to any particular experience, although it derives from
all the particular experiences we have had. It is a structure because it is
organized; it includes the relationships between its components parts.
As it may be inferred from the citation above, a schema consists of an abstract,
mentally-organized structure. It is abstract because it is not restricted to a single
particular experience we have had but to all of them. Furthermore, it is also
considered a structure because there is an organized relationship between its
constituents.
According to schema theory (Nuttall, 1996:7), all the assumptions made about the
world are closely related to experiences and the knowledge acquired from these
experiences. Empirical evidence (Stevens, 1980:151-153) has indicated that
previously possessed schemata/background knowledge consists of a prerequisite
if accurate reading is to result (ibid, p.151). Thus, whether readers are able to
interpret texts successfully will strongly depend on how similar their abstract
mentally-organized structures are to the writer’s. In addition, our schemata are
subject to constant processes of change since previous experiences tend to be
expanded throughout readers’ lives, i.e., the act of reading also affects the existing
schemata in the sense that readers will normally learn something new during the
actual reading processes. This view suggests that if, on the one hand, reading
15
makes use of existing schemata, on the other hand, reading simultaneously
modifies them (Nuttall, 1996:7).
These considerations are particularly valuable in improving understanding of what
being a proficient reader presupposes. Studies have demonstrated that proficient
readers are often aware of their existing schemata. Therefore, by engaging in the
active processing of the assumptions raised by the text, proficient readers activate
the relevant schemata so as to construct meanings. They are thus able to facilitate
their own processing of texts by resorting to both their content schemata (e.g.:
knowledge of content, socially-acquired knowledge, contextual background and
experience in reading) and their knowledge of writing conventions (e.g.: genre
features and register), i.e. their rhetorical schemata (Carrell, 1987: 467-9).
As Celce-Murcia & Olshtain (2000, 123-124) argue, proficient readers combine
rapid and accurate recognition and decoding of letters, words, collocations and other
structural cues, with sensible global predictions related to the text as a whole. They are
successful in selecting, abandoning, combining and adjusting strategies so as to
make sense of a given text. In addition, they are able to match old and new
information at global and local levels (Lopes, 1999) by linking words and ideas and
by focusing on the co-text. Proficient readers know how to make use of flexibility
(Kleiman, 2002:51). Not only do they know what their aim is but they are also able
to determine the choices in order to achieve their intent:
O leitor experiente tem duas características básicas que tornam a sua
leitura uma atividade consciente, reflexiva e intencional: primeiro, ele lê
porque tem algum objetivo em mente, isto é, sua leitura é realizada
sabendo para que está lendo, e, segundo, ele compreende o que lê, o que
16
seus olhos percebem seletivamente é interpretado, recorrendo a diversos
processos para tornar o texto inteligível quando não consegue
compreender
7
. (Kleiman, 2002:51).
A further element to be considered as far as textual comprehension is concerned,
is reader motivation for reading a particular text. Research developed in this area
has revealed that a reader’s needs and aims undoubtedly interfere and affect the
level of understanding.
Another aspect to be considered is that the tackling of different text types and
genres seem to require a variety of processes. Similarly, the same text may be
processed differently by people with different abilities and knowledge. Nuttall
(1996:7) claims that the way we interpret depends on the schemata activated by the text;
and whether we interpret successfully depends on whether our schemata are sufficiently
similar to the writer’s. It may therefore be inferred that interpretation is not solely
restricted to readers’ linguistic abilities. It is also culture-sensitive and demands a
combination of local and sociocultural knowledge (Wallace, 1992:28-29).
More recently, this view of reading has resulted in the so-called psycholinguistic-
cognitive approach (Barnett, 1989 apud Celce-Murcia & Olshstain, 2000:119) or
schema-oriented approach to reading. As Kleiman (2002:31) demonstrates, this
approach places particular emphasis on readers and prioritizes both cognitive
7
The experienced reader has two major basic characteristics which turn his/her reading into a
conscious, reflexive and purposeful activity: firstly, s/he reads because h/she has an objective in
mind, i.e., h/she knows why h/she is reading. Secondly, h/she understands what is being read: the
linguistic symbols perceived by the eyes are selected and interpreted by resorting to number of
processes in order to make sense of the text. [my translation]
17
development and text processing, i.e., readers’ interpretation strategies. The
schema-oriented approach to reading assumes that readers must have an adequate
schema for a topic before they are asked to read and comprehend material about that topic
(Stevens, 1980:153). The lack of adequate schema may affect comprehension
and eventually interfere and jeopardize learning. The implications of this latter view
of reading are particularly relevant to the development of effective reading lessons.
Once teachers are engaged in promoting effective understanding of target texts,
special attention must be devoted to the development of the prior knowledge bases upon
which sound reading depends (Stevens, 1980:153).
From this perspective, even though schemata constitute crucial elements
throughout the reading process, they must not be regarded as the unique source
of effective interpretation of texts. Thus, in an attempt to determine the mental
processes undergone by readers when attempting to make sense of texts,
researchers have focused on the development of reading models over the last two
decades.
2.1.3. Reading models: three distinct approaches to reading
Kleiman (2002:49) defines reading as an individual and lonely experience.
According to the author, reading consists of an individual and private activity to
construct meanings via the interaction between the writer and the reader. For this
reason, the same author claims that any attempt to teach reading appears to be
incoherent with its nature, since individuals are likely to differ in terms of
18
knowledge, interests and aims. However, Kleiman (op. cit.) favors the teaching of
reading as the teaching of a set of reading strategies, on one hand, and as the
development of linguistic skills on the other, even though these two factors are not
enough in themselves to promote efficient reading.
The same author argues that reading strategies consist of regular operations in
which readers engage when tackling a given text. Such operations may be inferred
by the researcher through the reconstruction and analysis of the mental activities
undergone by readers in order to comprehend a given text (e.g.: the reader’s
verbal and non-verbal behavior). In addition, elements such as the answers given
to the comprehension questions proposed, or the way readers summarize and
paraphrase ideas, as well as the way they manipulate the text (e.g.: underlining,
skimming, scanning, rereading etc), are all carefully considered as a means to
reconstruct the mental processes undergone by readers.
Research studies developed in the field of reading have demonstrated that the
same models developed in order to explain how readers of a particular discourse
community understand written texts in their first language may be applicable to
achieve an improved understanding of second/foreign language reading. As
Barnnet (1989:10, apud Lopes 1999:151) argues:
A model provides an imagined representation of the reading process that
not only provokes new ideas about reading but also provides a paradigm
against which aspects of the reading process may be tested.
19
Historically, research has identified three major categories on which the current
reading models are heavily based (Celce-Murcia & Olshtain, 2000: 119-123 &
Nuttall, 1996: 16-17): a) the bottom-up processing, b) the top-down processing
and c) the interactive. The first, according to Hudson (1998, apud Celce-Murcia &
Olshstain, 2000:119), regards reading as a series of stages that proceed in a fixed
order from sensory input to comprehension. It is essentially text-bound, relying heavily
on linguistic knowledge (e.g.: letters, the lexicon, phrases, and sentences). The
second however, views reading as a continuum of changing hypotheses about the
incoming information. It is mainly reader-bound, drawing upon the reader’s schemata
(i.e.: background knowledge, cognitive skills, memory and language proficiency)
as a means to confirm, reject or generate guesses. These two categories,
however, seem to be located in two opposing extremes. The former regards the
text as the only source of meaning and advocates a close relationship between
form (e.g.: the lexical items which constitute the text) and meaning – decodification
as semantic interpretation (Coracini, 2002:14). The latter, by the same token,
seems heavily linked to cognitive psychology and defines the “good reader” as
someone who is able to activate the knowledge stored in his/her mental schema
so as to make sense of the text. According to this view, readers are regarded as
the sole source of meaning and attend to the writer clues so that meaning is
conceived.
In the light of the insights generated from the two hypotheses described above,
Kato (1985, apud Coracini, 2002:13-14) advocates an intermediate theoretical
perspective of reading: the interactive view, whereby effective reading would
spring from the interaction writer-text-reader. Insights from research have revealed
20
that the interactive model of reading steams from an integration and combination of
both top-down and bottom-up approaches (Wallace, 1992:121) with the two models of
text processing occurring simultaneously:
... The reader needs to recruit his or her prior knowledge and prior
reading experience, apply knowledge of writing conventions, and
consider the purpose of reading in order to engage in top-down
processing. At the same time, the reader needs to recruit his or her
linguistic knowledge and various reading strategies in order to decode
the written text through bottom-up processing. It is the effective
integration of these processes that leads to the appropriate interpretation
and understanding of the written text and creates the interactive reading
process. (Celce-Murcia & Olshstain, 2000:121)
The interactive approach to reading, also defined as the socio-interactive
approach (Moita Lopes, 1996 apud Edmundson 2004:23) is heavily based on the
schema-oriented view of reading, whereby reading is viewed as a communicative
occurrence, i.e., writers and readers negotiate their mental schema through cues
spread within the text surface. From this perspective, meanings are reconstructed
through the reader’s direct contact with the text and indirect contact with the writer.
2.1.4. Reading skills and strategies
Even though results of research support the view that efficiency in reading is likely
to be enhanced through reading on a regular basis (Nuttall, 1996:40), there are
authors who advocate that learners may be taught how to read more efficiently
through the development of reading skills and strategies.
21
According to Grellet (1995:3) and Nuttall (1996:5), effective reading comprises
being able to interpret the writer’s communicative intent as efficiently as possible
through what Thorndike (1917: 323-332) described as a puzzle-solving process.
However, interpreting the writer’s intentions constitutes more than the
straightforward task it may at first seem. Readers need to be attentive to the
writers’ moves towards organizing their discourse, i.e., they must be aware of the
various tools at their disposal in order to grasp the meaning which emerges from
the printed pages. Efficient readers must be able to recognize a multiplicity of
linguistic signals including letters, morphemes, syllables, words, phrases,
grammatical cues, discourse markers and by using their linguistic data processing
mechanisms decide what needs to be retained or discarded. In the psycholinguistic
guessing game (Goodman, 1976:126-135), the audience must be aware of the fact
that no text carries meaning by itself i.e., the sense of a given text also lies in the
reader. Texts consist of interactive pieces of language from which meanings may
derive only and if readers know what needs to retained or rejected.
Kleiman (2002:50) classifies the strategies employed by readers into two major
groups: a) cognitive strategies and b) metacognitive strategies. The same author
claims that cognitive reading strategies comprise the operations of which readers
are not aware. They are unconscious operations (Kleiman, 2002:50) used by readers
in order to approach a given text. By unconscious, is meant implicit knowledge over
which individuals fail to have explicit control and, as a consequence, is hard to be
conveyed into words. These procedures are also labeled automatism (Kleiman,
2002:50).
22
In contrast to cognitive reading strategies, metacognitive strategies are those
operations (not rules) realized with a particular objective in mind and over which we
possess explicit and conscious control in the sense that we are able to express and explain
our action (Kleiman, 2002:50). It may be inferred, therefore, that readers who are
able to perform their mental activities consciously i.e., who have overt control over
the operations developed while reading will be able to determine when they cannot
understand a particular piece of writing and what the source of difficulty might be.
As Kleiman (2002:50) suggests, proficient readers are aware of the activities in
which they should engage so as to read more efficiently (e.g.: going back and
rereading certain fragments, looking for and focusing on key-words, summarizing
what has just been read, looking for concepts and examples, etc). Nuttall
(1996:40) corroborates Kleiman’s views by defining strategy training as a useful
means towards raising learners’ awareness of the importance of being able to
make conscious choices when tackling difficult texts. Similarly, learners must be
taught how to tackle texts independently, to detect the sources of potential
difficulties as well as to adopt appropriate strategies in order to interpret meanings
(Nuttall, 1996:41).
Thus, within an effective reading program, teachers must try to anticipate the
possible sources of difficulties for learners. Such difficulties may spring from
vocabulary, syntax, interpretation of value, relationships between different parts of
a text or between reader, writer and text, cultural differences among other sources.
This justifies the relevance of equipping learners with a wide range of strategies to
deal with texts (Nuttall, 1996:41). The range of strategies to approach texts are
regarded as attack skills (Nuttall, op. cit.) and are subdivided into word attack skills
23
and text attack skills. As far as these two types of skills are concerned, Nuttall
(1996:41) argues the following:
Most students are well aware when they have problems with vocabulary
so they usually appreciate the need for word attack skills. Other sources
of difficulty tend to be less familiar; training in text attack skills therefore
involves making students aware of things they normally do not notice at
all. This awareness is crucial; readers who understand what can go
wrong can take steps to sort out their difficulty.
However, if, on one hand, learners ought to be aware that difficulties may arise
from bits of the text (Nuttall, 1996:41), and that they should be ready to detect
such difficulties as well as cope with them, on the other hand, learners have to be
aware of the fact that the various text features are not fully separable i.e., they are
linked to one another. As Nuttall (op. cit.) claims:
The reader must be able to respond to its overall message, evaluate the
writer’s success, and if appropriate appreciate the text as literature.
In general terms, it may be inferred thus that the processes of reading comprise a
wide range of cognitive and metacognitive strategies to approach texts.
Consequently, the ideal approach to teaching reading seems to embody a
combination of both types of skills. Reading efficiently springs from: a) knowing
what you want from reading, b) choosing the right material and b) using the text
effectively (Nuttall, 1996:46). As highlighted in the preceding section, these three
factors facilitate the identification of what is relevant according to one’s purpose.
Proficient readers know how to make good use of flexibility:
People who read flexibly are skilled at judging what they need to get out
of a text to accomplish their purpose. (...) Reading flexibly means always
24
keeping in mind how much you need to read in order to satisfy your
purpose. This helps you to decide which parts of the text to ignore, which
to skim to get the gist, and then which parts to study closely. (Nuttall,
1996:48)
2.1.5. The three major constituents of the reading process
As pointed out on the previous pages, research studies over decades have
acknowledged reading as an interactive process by investigating its three major
constituents in order to achieve a more comprehensive understanding of its
nature. According to several authors, reading comprises three essential and
influential elements, namely the reader, the text and the writer (Alderson &
Urquhart, 1984: XV). These three elements must be integrated throughout what is
defined by Wallace (1992:44-45) as a dialogic relation, whereby readers are
actively involved towards extracting the required information from texts as
efficiently as possible. The selection of effective strategies, however, is likely to be
determined by the purpose(s) for reading (Grellet, 1981:3). Nuttall (1996:3)
describes such reasons as authentic. By authentic the author means reasons that are
concerned not with language learning but with the uses of reading in our daily lives
outside the classroom.
Kleiman (2001:3) further corroborates Wallace’s views on reading by arguing that
it consists of a complex process which demands the combination of a diversity of
factors (e.g.: attention, perception, memory etc) so that meanings may be
constructed. According to the author, reading no longer constitutes a linear and
serial process (Kleiman, 2001:17) since the acceptance or rejection of the ideas
25
presented appear to be closely related to a wide body of knowledge which
interacts so as to promote comprehension. This moderate view of reading rejects
the somewhat simplistic, often-held view, which suggests that meanings are lying
there in the text passively waiting to be “extracted” or “absorbed” by the reader. As
a consequence, in the last two decades, a multitude of research studies about the
act of reading have placed special emphasis on the reader (Alderson & Urquhart,
1984: XVI). These investigations addressed as “reader-focused studies” aimed at
analyzing the reading skill into a series of subskills. From such perspective, the act
of reading would be viewed as a set of subskills that would be related to one another
within a taxonomy or hierarchy of skills (Alderson & Urquhart, 1984: XVI).
Alderson & Urquhart (1984: XVI) highlight that a diversity of approaches to
determine the subskills applied by readers have been developed. However, in their
view, the most usual approach consists in providing learners with a series of
passages to be understood. Subsequently, learners are asked questions, which
are later subjected to factor analysis so as to determine whether identifiable
factors emerge. These investigations have resulted in a wide range of
taxonomies/lists with varied content. Among such taxonomies, Alderson
addresses that by Barrett (1968: XVI) which comprises five skills, namely literal
comprehension, reorganization of the ideas in the text, inferential ability, evaluation, and
appreciation.
Davies and Widdowson (1974, apud Alderson & Urquhart 1984: XVI) developed a
list of reading comprehension questions, particularly relevant to the testing of
reading skills. This list comprises direct reference questions, inference, supposition and
26
evaluation questions. Textbooks aimed at teaching reading as a foreign language
usually contain a variety of texts followed by comprehension questions. Such
questions are usually designed based on a particular taxonomy which has as its
ultimate goal to test the learner’s ability to understand the text at a variety of
levels. These levels, by the same token, are supposed to reflect the taxonomies
established by research studies.
2.1.6. Proficient readers: a combination of reading skills and strategies
As far as the processing of texts is concerned, Nuttall (1996:44) defines efficiency
as the ability to use the least effort to obtain satisfactory results. It is certainly true that
for EFL teachers in general, the concept of efficiency in reading seems heavily
connected to the idea of understanding a given text. In fact, as Nuttall (op. cit.)
argues, efficiency in reading springs from a combination of two conflicting factors:
comprehension and the amount of time devoted to the actual reading. Similarly,
these two factors are also strongly related to the audience’s purpose in tackling a
particular text. Nuttall (op. cit.) argues that the primary requirement for EFL or FL
efficient reading is that readers be aware of what is expected from their own
reading. In other words, defining an ultimate goal for tackling a particular text
seems fundamental. Whether one is reading for practical purposes (i.e., checking
how to operate a machine, for example) or not, the aim has to be clearly defined.
Efficiency is likely to occur due to purposeful/meaningful reading. It may therefore
be inferred that EFL teachers are likely to enhance the reading efficiency rate
among learners by encouraging them to define their ultimate goals prior to tackling
a particular text either in class or in other contexts. However, in order to determine
27
clear purposes for classroom reading as well as define which texts to read and
how to read them, it is important to carefully consider the needs and aims
conceived within the classroom boundaries.
According to Grellet (1981), effective reading seems to derive from an integration
between linguistic knowledge and the ability to use and choose from a variety of
sub-skills and strategies such as skimming, scanning, intensive reading, extensive
reading etc. Furthermore, text-types and aims seem to guide readers towards
determining which strategies would be more appropriate depending on the target
text. Celce-Murcia & Olshstain (2000:119) claim that throughout the process of
trying to make sense of a particular text, the reader performs a variety of
simultaneous tasks such as: a) decoding the message by recognizing the written signs,
b) interpreting the message by assigning meaning to the string of words, and finally, c)
understanding what the author’s intention was. Again, throughout this process, the
three key participants namely the writer, text and reader are actively involved. This
corroborates the assumption of reading as an inherently interactive process (Celce-
Murcia & Olshstain, 2000:119).
A further view regards effective reading as a reduction-of-uncertainty process
(Smith, 1971). According to this view, individuals, as they progress through the
target text, resort to a variety of choices determined by features within and outside
the text. Once the objective for reading is defined and the material to be read has
been chosen, it is time to proceed to the textual resources which are of substantial
value towards reading efficiently.
28
Successful reading may spring from a complementary combination of bottom-up
(skimming) and top-down (scanning) processing strategies. The former engages
readers in analyzing the graphic display, decoding words and structures, in order
to build up concepts and, as a consequence, construct meanings. The latter
appeals to the reader’s “stored knowledge” as a means to assimilate or reject the
assumptions presented in the text. These two approaches to reading (Nuttall,
1996:16-17 & Kato, apud Coracini, 2002:13) consist of prediction strategies which
enable readers to anticipate what will follow across whole texts and within
sentences/paragraphs (Nuttall: 1996:12-15). This view stresses the notion of
inference as a crucial element in the effective processing of texts. Collins, Brown
and Larkin (1979:386) define inference as follows:
... Inference is thought of as filling in the missing connections between
the surface structure fragments of the text by recourse to context and
knowledge of the world.
.... the purpose of inference is to select and fill out a set of frames or
scripts or schemas.
Unlike unskilled readers, skilled readers are able to move beyond the surface
level, not being tied to the sequential structure. They deal with the difficulties
encountered by resorting to a range of problem-solving strategies for evaluating,
revising and confirming their initial hypotheses. For them, text understanding is
developed through a progressive refinement of initial hypotheses constructed from
schemas triggered by the beginning elements of the text. After incorporating a
wider range of elements from the text, these initial hypotheses are progressively
refined in order to fill in the slots which still remain unfilled until other meanings are
constructed.
29
Lopes (1999:150) advocates that an interactive approach to texts regards
skimming and scanning as complementary and equally important for successful
reading. Furthermore, efficient readers also need to be able to make use of other
resources within the text in order to achieve comprehension. Among these
resources, the following may be cited: a) linear texts and b) non-linear texts. The
former corresponds to the expression of ideas by means of sentences within the
space of paragraphs whereas the latter consists of elements not organized in
sentences or paragraphs but which enable the understanding of linear texts
(Nuttall, 1996:45). Graphic conventions (e.g.: symbols, layout and punctuation),
reference apparatus (e.g.: blurbs, indexes and titles) and figures (e.g.: illustrations,
graphs and tables) constitute examples of non-linear texts.
Proficient readers benefit from their knowledge of both linear and non-linear texts
and, as a consequence, are able to construct meanings more efficiently. It may
thus be inferred that a combination of a variety of reading strategies plays a
valuable part in the development of efficiency in reading. Within this scenario, the
schema theory emerges as a crucial concept towards understanding how human
beings in general tend to organize the previously-acquired knowledge which
enables them to interpret texts.
The theoretically-based discussion developed in the preceding paragraphs seems
to corroborate the view that the effective development of reading skills should
consider a wide range of variables. Thus, due to the purposes of the present
30
research investigation, the issues surrounding the teaching of reading within
overseas settings will be addressed in the next sub-section.
2.1.7. The relevance of EFL reading within a global scenario
In a literate society, skill in reading is imperative since so much of what
one needs to know is communicated via written text (...) We could not
function in modern society without reading. Yet for some people this is
not an easily accessible skill. (Celce– Murcia & Olshstain, 2000:118)
As Celce-Murcia & Olshtain highlight, reading appears to consist of an ability of
substantial relevance in our modern global scenario. As far as second and foreign
languages are concerned, the same authors (2000:118) claim that reading carries
even greater potential importance since this skill is often the only exposure readers
have to the target language. They aver that learners of other languages are likely
to retain a substantial amount of the knowledge acquired throughout a course of
study by subsequent reading in that language on a regular basis. This practice
enables learners to retain the knowledge gained for many years after completing
their language courses. Furthermore, through reading, learners of languages of
remote geographical areas have the opportunity to access the world of literature
and culture representing such regions.
Individuals read for a number of reasons and for a variety of purposes. Alderson
(1984:1) claims that foreign language reading knowledge, for instance, is often
important, not only for academic studies, but also for professional success, in
addition to personal development. This is particularly true in the case of the
English language in Brazil, which is widely used in a range of contemporary
31
professional and scientific fields, websites and publications. Critically, it is the
internet which has become the major tool of English as a means of communicating
worldwide.
However, even though the advantages of being able to read in a foreign language
for academic or professional purposes seem unquestionable, it is still often argued
that the most frequent justification for the teaching of reading, within EFL settings,
is that learners read in order to consolidate the spoken language. This is
particularly true at the elementary and intermediate levels of language learning, in
which it is still often common practice to provide learners with texts which contain
examples of the structures and lexical items recently-acquired and practiced orally.
The question then is whether the texts provided for learners in such circumstances
reflect the language encountered in “real” texts. By “real” it is meant “authentic”
i.e., texts which have specific, accepted, and recognized communicative functions,
and which feature a wide range of structures, lexical items, registers, cohesive
devices, formats and textual organization which authentic texts often contain. A
further element to be considered is that the range of reading skills and approaches
which are developed when tackling “authentic” texts will usually differ from the
approaches often adopted when reading is introduced as a means of consolidating
EFL oral abilities. “Real” reading presupposes a combination of a variety of
reading skills such as reading for gist (skimming), for specific information
(scanning) and for inference. These aspects of meaningful reading are to be
discussed in the subsequent sections of the present chapter.
32
As far as the relevance of EFL reading is concerned, it is surely difficult to
determine when and where EFL learners will make use of English as a vehicle of
communication outside the classroom. However, when they eventually need to do
so it is likely to be in reading since, in Brazil, most up-dated literature (e.g.:
abstracts, journals, periodicals, newspapers, websites etc), in a variety of
professional fields, is available exclusively in English. Another aspect to be
considered is that by equipping learners with the reading skills required to
approach a given text in the target language they may even be helped to be more
effective L1 readers, through the transference of skills.
In view of the considerations presented above, it may be argued that restricting
reading to purely classroom/pedagogic purposes is not advisable. However, if this
is the case, learners must be provided with practice in reading, rather than simply
consolidating oral competence. For this reason, prior to starting any reading
activity, it is important to establish the goals, as well as the means towards
achieving them. Selecting the types of texts to be dealt with inside the classroom
is of crucial importance since different text types make different demands on the reader
(Nuttall, 1996:30). Furthermore, the tasks and activities to be proposed to learners
should also be a focal point upon which teachers should reflect. The development
of effective reading skills in a second or foreign language seems to require a focus
on the difficulties encountered by nonnative speakers when tackling texts in the
target language.
2.1.8. Approaching reading within overseas settings
Language improvement is a natural by-product of reading, and a highly
desirable one. (Nuttall, 1996:30)
33
Nuttall (1996:30) asserts that reading appears to be a highly effective means of
expanding one’s mastery of the target language. However, as far as reading
lessons are concerned, FL teachers are well-advised to aim at motivating learners
to read for meaning. According to the same author, a substantial number of FL
learners read for a number of reasons which are not closely related to language
learning, but to the conveying of a given message (e.g.: dealing with overseas
customers, keeping up to date with research in nuclear physics, etc). From this
perspective, language constitutes no more than the vehicle through which
messages are expressed. As a result, it is of crucial relevance to focus on the
importance of written texts as vehicles to convey meanings. This discussion is
particularly important due to the fact that messages within texts, very often, tend to
be regarded by teachers as vehicles providing no more than samples of the target
language (e.g.: lexical items and structures) to be acquired by learners.
According to Nuttall (1996:30), there are two types of lessons in which the use of
written texts is central: text-based lessons and reading lessons. The same author
defines the major distinction between both types of lessons as follows:
...Giving a lesson based on a text (i.e.: a text-based lesson) is not the
same thing as giving a reading lesson: most of the skills practiced are
probably not reading skills at all. (Nuttall, op. cit.)
As it may be inferred from the quotation above, according to Nuttall (op.cit.), text-
based lessons and reading lessons differ in two ways:
(a) The first distinction highlights that text types chosen for reading lessons, and
text-based lessons, are likely to differ. In the former, texts are used do convey
34
a message in the widest sense of the term (e.g.: to inform, to entertain, etc.).
In contrast, in the latter, teachers seem to be particularly concerned about
language development. Meanings are thus subordinate to, for example, the
acquisition of lexical items and grammatical structures.
(b) The second distinction lies in the fact that the teacher’s aims and procedures
adopted in a reading lesson are likely to differ from those of a text-based
lesson. As explained above, in a text-based lesson the text is used as a
vehicle to focus on structures or vocabulary of the target language. In a
reading lesson, on the other hand, meanings are central (Nuttall, 1996:30).
Learners are therefore required to derive messages from a particular piece of
writing, which suggests that in a reading lesson any new learning of
vocabulary or structures emerges as an incidental benefit (Nuttall, 1996:30).
A further consideration to be made as far as reading lessons are concerned is that
readers’ aims are likely to differ from one another. Thus there is no fixed recipe for a
reading lesson (ibid, p. 30-31). Reading lessons should cater for both the variety of
texts and the variety of readers (ibid, p. 31). Nuttall defines the general goal of an
efficient reading program along the following lines:
To enable students to enjoy (or at least feel comfortable with) reading in
the foreign language, and to read without help unfamiliar authentic texts,
at appropriate speed, silently and with adequate understanding. (Nuttall,
1996:31)
In very general terms Nuttall’s (op.cit.) definition above provides good food for
thought in the sense that it seems to carry valuable implications for the teaching of
reading within overseas settings. Nuttall (op. cit.) explains the definition as follows:
35
(a) To enable students: As Nuttall (op.cit.) asserts: In a reading lesson, it is what
the student does, not what the teacher does, that counts. Teachers and learners
possess well-defined roles. The former are expected to promote the
reading ability in the learners, whereas the latter must be aware of the
importance of their actual involvement throughout the development of
lessons.
(b) To enjoy (or at least feel comfortable with): Teachers must choose interesting
and relevant reading materials to be used in class so that reading lessons
become enjoyable learning opportunities.
(c) To read without help: Since real life reading is usually a lonely act (Kleiman,
2002:49), learners must be encouraged to become independent readers.
They must be able to tackle a wide variety of texts without counting on their
teachers’ support.
(d) Unfamiliar texts: Even though students have both teachers’ and fellow
students’ support throughout their training in class, they must be equipped
with the skills necessary to tackle unfamiliar texts, since these are the texts
they are likely to encounter in real-life situations.
(e) Authentic texts: As mentioned before, teachers should be aware of the
needs and aims of their target groups so that students are equipped with
the reading skills required for real-life purposes. Thus, teachers should be
sensitive in order to determine at which stage students have sufficient skills
36
so as to be introduced to the target texts. As Nuttall (1996:31) claims, some of
the practice should be with target texts, i.e, the sort of texts the students will want to
read after they have completed their course.
(f) Appropriate speed: Students should be provided with practice in determining
which type of reading best suits their purposes and target texts. Students
should be encouraged to take risks and to adopt a flexible reading style
which, as Nuttall (1996:32) argues, is the sign of a competent reader.
(g) Silently: Proficient readers should be able to read silently. Therefore,
students should be equipped with this skill which, in most cases, tends to
be overlooked or ignored, in the early stages of reading.
(h) With adequate understanding: the notion of being able to read efficiently is
not solely restricted to the understanding of every single word within a text
(Nutall, 1996:32). This suggests that rather than simply focusing on
precision, teachers should equip learners with this indispensable skill so as
to tackle texts according to their purposes for reading such texts.
As far as the teaching and learning of reading is concerned, it appears to be
common practice to acknowledge coursebooks as the sole major tool for
promoting meaningful learning within EFL classrooms. This view may be regarded
as partially true in view of the fact that coursebooks are usually the only material
available in a wide range of teaching/learning environments. Thus, the issues
surrounding the relevance of coursebooks appear to be central, as far as the
37
teaching of reading is concerned. In the next subsection, therefore, a number of
considerations will be made concerning the instrumental validity of coursebooks.
In addition, there will be an attempt to describe a more critical approach to
coursebooks, which is likely to enlighten pedagogical practices.
2.1.9. An enlightened approach to EFL reading via “alien” coursebooks
A crutch, a tool, a doorway, a bible, a route map, a drug. According to research
(Bolitho, 2004:7), these are a selection of the metaphorical terms utilized by
teachers in general in order to define the love-hate relationship they experience in
relation to coursebooks. However, most teachers seem to agree that coursebooks
appear to play a crucial role in the teaching/learning process and, as a
consequence, in their professional lives. As Santos (2004:32) avers, muitos
professores procuram encontrar legitimidade e apoio para suas aulas neste material
8
.
This importance given to coursebooks may be historically explained. It dates back
to the Middle Ages when the advent of printing enabled the expansion of
knowledge in a way that was, until then, unimaginable. As a result, education
became more accessible and textbooks (i.e., literally books of texts) were
regarded as the major tools within the educational scenario. Similarly, it was
assumed that learners who were able to reproduce the content of textbooks would
8
A wide range of teachers attempt to find theoretical support and reliance on coursebooks [my
translation]
38
inevitably succeed. Coursebooks were assumed to contain everything a student
needs to know in order to pass an examination (Bolitho, 2004:7). According to the
same author, even though this belief still constitutes a tradition in certain cultures,
the situation appears to have undergone significant transformations in the last
three decades.
As far as ELT is concerned, the advent of the communicative approach has
promoted a shift in focus. A new generation of coursebooks focused on
communicative principles was created and the focus shifted from the acquisition of
knowledge to the acquisition of skills. These new coursebooks are no longer
“textbooks” but rather sources of stimuli for skills development and communication
(Bolitho, 2004:7).
Moreover, the revolution in worldwide communications has enabled publishers to
expand their selling of coursebooks to teachers and learners from the most remote
geographical areas. As the demand for coursebooks increased, publishing
companies were expected to meet these new demands. As a result, a fierce
competition to gain buyers was triggered between publishing companies fighting to
launch new, more competitive publications (Bolitho: 2004:8). This is particularly
true in the case of EFL teaching/learning. The design and selling of coursebooks
has become a profitable business, and booksellers, coursebook writers and
publishers have benefited enormously from the expansion of English-teaching
markets. As a result of the growing interest for the English language, there has
been much debate, not only on the appropriateness and effectiveness of FL
teaching methods, but also on the teaching materials available and how
39
appropriate they have been in terms of the numerous and different learners’
purposes, needs and contexts. Within the Brazilian EFL teaching/learning
scenario, the choice of coursebooks is very often imposed on teachers, who are
seldom provided with the opportunity to make their own choices (Santos,
2004:32). Furthermore, there seems to be a tendency to regard coursebooks as
the sole source from where consolidated knowledge may emerge. This is
particularly true, as far as EFL coursebooks produced abroad are concerned:
No caso do LD de LE, há também a crença de que principalmente o
autor nativo é aquele que detêm a “fonte do saber” em sua totalidade.
Acreditamos que a legitimidade do livro está justamente relacionada à
crença de que ele seja o detentor da verdade, a qual se encontra
“pronta” para ser transmitida e compartilhada. O papel do professor
seria então reproduzir as idéias dos autores, enquanto aos alunos
caberia assimilá-las
9
. (Santos, 2004:33)
More recently, it seems to make common sense that the choice of coursebooks
should spring from a variety of pre-established principles, which are expected to
consider target learners, contexts and aims. The adoption of materials, mainly
those produced overseas, should emerge from a concern with the learning
process as a whole. The implications of adopting non-sensitive teaching practice
and materials which disregard the learners’ needs, objectives and contexts should
be carefully considered. Santos (2004:34) argues that teachers are expected to be
able to determine to what extent coursebooks are incompatible with the target
9
As far as EFL coursebooks are concerned, there seems to be a tendency to view coursebooks
produced by native-speakers as the only source of “true knowledge”, which is ready to be passed
on to teachers and learners. From such a perspective, the language teacher’s role would be reduced
to the mere repetition of the concepts and beliefs imposed by writers and publishing companies.
Similarly, the learner’s role would be restricted to the assimilation of this “true knowledge”. [my
translation]
40
audience’s specificities and, at a further stage, find ways to tackle possible
mismatches:
(...) salientamos ser de suma importância que professores sejam capazes
de identificar possíveis incompatibilidades do LD em face das
peculiaridades de sala de aula
10
(Santos, 2004:34)
In view of the considerations discussed above, it might be inferred that the choice
of coursebooks implies a sense of judgment. Coursebooks should not be judged
by their cover (Cunningsworth, 1989). Instead, teachers should behave critically
towards the material being examined and should not take what is in its introduction
as universal truths. It is their role to question and check whether the content
matches the author’s claims in the introduction. Moreover, teachers should bear in
mind that books are produced to be sold and no author would be naive enough to
inform them that learners might not achieve their aims by the end of the course.
All in all, it might be argued that investigations developed in the editorial arena
(Bolitho, 2004:10) suggest that the publishing industry seems to have made a very
positive contribution to the development of ELT. Publishing companies have been
responsible for the publishing of a wide range of outstanding global coursebooks.
Nevertheless, with this success come responsibilities which are gradually being
recognized, more in some contexts than in others. If we are to avoid worldwide
“Mcdonalization” in ELT, the lessons of the last decade need to be learned (Bolitho,
2004:10).
10
We claim that it is of crucial importance that teachers be able to identify potential mismatches
between coursebooks and the specificities imposed by the classroom boundaries. [my translation]
41
The avoidance of “Mcdonalization” (Bolitho, 2004:10) in ELT appears to embody
the adoption of a more enlightened and critically-oriented approach to
coursebooks. Therefore, in order to choose coursebooks which reflect the
learners’ needs, aims and contexts, as well as the most recent pedagogical trends
in reading, it seems fundamental that teachers be aware of what is meant by
reading and what it involves. Moreover, the complexity inherent in the act of
reading also suggests that the testing of reading should also consist of a complex
task. In the next section the issues surrounding the testing of reading will be
addressed.
42
2.2. THE TESTING OF READING
2.2.1. Introduction
The role English plays as the language of the Information and
Communication Age is prompting a rethinking of approaches to the
teaching of reading in many parts of the world (Richards, 2005:10).
Reading lessons are expected to engage learners in developing their analytical
processing skills, problem solving and critical thinking (Richards, 2005:10).
Furthermore, the development of technical reading skills should also be prioritized
in any reading program. This relatively novel view of reading seems to directly
reflect what happens inside and outside EFL classrooms. Thus, it might be
inferred that the new approaches to the teaching of reading seem to have brought
relevant implications for the testing of reading.
In this section, the issues surrounding the testing of reading skills will be
examined. The aim here is to provide a picture of the underlying principles which
seem to govern the elaboration of the reading comprehension papers in the FCE
examination. This discussion is justified by the fact that the testing of reading is
often seen as a source of concern among EFL teachers in general. It is regarded
as a demanding task since very often teachers feel insecure about how the
development of the reading skills should be assessed.
However, prior to engaging in the discussion on how reading should be assessed
it seems important to verify the underlying principles which are claimed to nurture
the elaboration of standardized tests. In the following subsection a selection of
43
considerations will be made regarding one of the elements of the corpus of
analysis, namely the FCE examination.
2.2.2. Background to the FCE
2.2.2.1. General considerations regarding UCLES
The UCLES was established as a department of the University of Cambridge in
1858. At that time, its major concern was to establish a standard of efficiency for
British schools. More recently, the UCLES has become responsible for both the
professional and specialist aspects of EFL examinations and TEFL schemes.
The EFL examinations were first introduced in 1913 with the CPE, which was
followed by the FCE, first administered in 1939. There is today, a wide range of
EFL examinations administered by the UCLES in different parts of the globe.
These examinations cover a variety of both academic and vocational subjects
11
.
The question paper production process consists the core of the UCLES EFL
system (UCLES handbook, 2001:4). This process involves a specialized staff so
as to ensure that the pre-established standards are being met and that the
examinations develop in accordance with the ongoing changing needs of both
candidates and other test users. Moreover, the UCLES EFL system is also in
charge of other three major areas further outlined:
11
It seems important to highlight that several EFL examinations are claimed to be especially
designed for the international market.
44
(a) Providing support for the administration of the examinations: the
development of training and monitoring procedures indispensable for the
adequate assessment of spoken and written language by the examiners;
(b) The processing of examinations (e.g.: marking, etc.);
(c) The user service: production of information (e.g.: handbooks, sample
materials, examination reports, etc.) for test users.
Apart from the responsibilities previously described, the UCLES EFL staff is also in
charge of ensuring that the test users’ duties are being met so that the UCLES
EFL examinations reflect the Code of Practice determined by the ALTE
12
. This
association was founded in 1990 and has as its major concern the responsibilities
of both examination providers and examination users in four distinct domains
(UCLES handbook, 2001:4):
(a) developing examinations;
(b) interpreting examination results;
(c) striving for fairness;
(d) informing examination takers.
2.2.2.2. The FCE
UCLES has developed a variety of EFL examinations which conform to five
different levels. The FCE, according to the UCLES handbook (2001:6) is the most
widely taken Cambridge EFL examination (i.e., 270,000 people each year in more
12
All the members of the ALTE are providers of language examinations and certificates from
countries within the European Union.
45
than 100 countries) and corresponds to Cambridge Level Three which may be
observed in table 2.1. below:
The FCE is an intermediate level Cambridge ESOL exam which demands from
candidates a command of the target language which is good enough to construct
arguments through appropriate communication styles in accordance with a wide
range of situations. Moreover, FCE candidates are expected to show an
awareness of register and conventions of politeness and degrees of formality.
Successful candidates receive a certificate awarded by The University of
Cambridge which is said to be recognized by universities and employers in
different parts of the world. Along with the certificate, passing candidates receive a
statement of results showing their performance in each of the five papers (i.e.
reading, writing, use of English, listening and speaking) which constitute the FCE.
Moreover, prior to undergoing the FCE, candidates are recommended to attend
exam practice courses aimed at preparing for the five papers. In Brazil such
Cambridge Level Five
Certificate of Proficiency in English (CPE)
Cambridge Level Four
Certificate in Advanced English (CAE)
Cambridge Level Three
First Certificate in English (FCE)
Cambridge Level Two
Preliminary English Test (PET)
Cambridge Level One
Key English Test (KET)
Table 2.1.: (Cambridge EFL examinations. From UCLES handbook, 2001:6)
46
courses are often provided by private language schools which charge for the
lessons. Furthermore, even though exam practice courses tend to be regarded as
an essential element at the preparation stage, attending the exam practice course
constitutes no guarantee of achieving positive results. Furthermore, it almost goes
without saying that the institutions which offer such courses have made
considerable profit from monthly payments.
However, it seems worth questioning the validity of exam practice courses since
candidates are assumed to have already developed the four skills by the end of
their language courses. From this perspective, obtaining positive results in ESOL
exams would come as an additional benefit, besides becoming proficient in the
target language. As far as the testing of reading is concerned, FCE candidates are
expected to be familiar with both the format and types of questions proposed on
the exam. Thus, it seems worth focusing at this stage on the distinctions, if any,
between teaching and testing.
2.2.3. Similarities between teaching reading and testing reading
Alderson (1996 apud Nuttall, 1996: chapter 13) highlights that the literature on the
testing of reading has revealed that the distinction between the teaching and
testing of reading is not as wide as some teachers may believe. On the contrary,
research studies (Richards, 2005:11) have demonstrated that the teaching and
testing of reading are often somewhat similar in certain ways:
In many classrooms there was little difference in approach between
teaching reading and testing reading. (Richards, 2005:11)
47
In the same chapter, Alderson (ibid, p. 212) argues that the main distinction
between teaching and testing reading resides in the way materials are
manipulated rather than in the materials themselves. As far as the design of
exercises and classroom activities are concerned, for instance, the author claims
that there is no clear cut difference between the design of tasks presented in
textbooks and test items. Among the major reading comprehension questions
identified in reading comprehension sections from coursebooks, Edmundson
(2005:32-37) highlights a) true or false questions, b) yes/no questions, c) multiple
choice(s), d) copy (finding specific information), e) wh questions, f) matching the
columns, g) fill in the blanks h) inference i) referential and j) metalinguistic (i.e.:
focused on formal aspects such as structure/grammar and lexicon). In general,
coursebooks are claimed to contain tasks and exercises which resemble tests and
vice-versa.
2.2.4. Differences between teaching and testing reading
Alderson (1996 apud Nuttall, 1996, chapter 13) seems to be particularly concerned
with the differences between the testing and teaching of reading skills. According
to the author (1996 apud Nuttall, 1996:212), the testing and teaching of reading
differ in four ways:
The first distinction resides in the fact that examinees receive little or no support
when they undergo a test. The exercises aimed at testing are regarded as a
means to assess to what extent learners are able to perform on their own. This
sharply contrasts with what happens when teachers provide learners with a text as
48
a teaching activity. In this circumstance, having support from teachers constitutes
normal practice since the teachers’ role is to facilitate learning. Thus, they
administer activities through a variety of techniques (e.g.: by repeating previous
lesson points or activities or explaining vocabulary and grammatical points) so as
to engage learners in grasping the intended messages and responding to the
tasks proposed appropriately.
In contrast, when performing a test, learners are expected to demonstrate how
well they can perform without the teacher’s assistance. And, even though
collaboration constitutes a crucial element throughout the learning process, it is
expected to be discouraged during a test on the grounds that it provides a false
picture of the learner’s abilities to deal with the tasks successfully (ibid, p. 213).
Since examinees are expected to perform on their own, test designers play an
essential role. The instructions provided on tests should be straightforward and
unambiguous (ibid). Moreover, the tasks proposed should be familiar to learners;
i.e., they should resemble the type of questions provided in class.
The second distinction between the teaching and testing of reading is that the
latter is intended to discriminate; i.e., tests are supposed to be hard for a particular
group of learners. Tests are expected to establish the difference between learners
who are able to perform the tasks successfully and those who are not. The tests
which discriminate between students are usually addressed as “norm-referenced
tests”. According to Lopes (1999:177), in this type of test, an individual score is
49
weighed against all the scores in the testing group. In addition, such tests are intended
to be standardized.
Calkins, Montgomery and Santman (1998 apud Lopes, 1999, chapter 6) criticize
the supremacy of standardized norm-referenced tests: The problem with this is that
the test development process is guided by the attempt to sort and rank students in relation
to each other. It is not guided by an effort to reflect what students actually know and can
do. They claim that by focusing on such tests, the teacher’s capacity to evaluate
their learners as well as to devise “informed techniques” and apply curriculum based
pedagogy in order to enable pupils to become conscious citizens (op. cit., p.178) seem to
be underestimated. The authors highlight that these tests are made deliberately
difficult. Therefore, most examinees do not manage to answer the questions
proposed and no more than a small percentage of them are likely to obtain
positive results.
Another aspect discussed by Calkins, Montgomery and Santman (op.cit.) is
related to the secrecy of the curriculum which governs the rationale of standardized
norm-referenced tests (Lopes, 1999:178). These tests are very often regarded as
being “curriculum free” or “curriculum neutral”, i.e., they may be applied to
everyone regardless of cultural, economic or social differences. In addition, the
scores obtained in standardized norm-referenced tests also serve as a means to
evaluate the teacher’s capacity, the principals and the schools. Therefore, low
scores, are often thought to indicate that the foreign language teaching curriculum
needs to be improved.
50
Unlike norm-referenced tests, criterion-referenced or mastery tests are expected to
reveal the learners’ strengths rather than their weaknesses. In such tests the
general public (i.e.: students, teachers and principals) are aware of the criteria
adopted for the correction as well as of the knowledge and skills to be assessed
(Calkins, Montgomery and Santman, 1998, apud Lopes, 1999, chapter 6).
Administering progress or achievement tests would be another alternative towards
focusing on what learners were expected to learn: Success breeds success and failure
breeds failure (Alderson, apud Nuttall, 1996:213). Thus designing tests which are
too difficult for learners is likely to be discouraging and frustrating. It may lead
those learners to question their ability to learn the target language or reject it
based on the misconception that it is above their level of understanding. For the
reasons previously mentioned, Alderson (ibid) argues that it often makes pedagogic
sense to make tests deliberately easy.
The third difference between the tasks and activities used in order to teach and
test reading is that the latter constitute representative samples (ibid, p. 214) of the
work done in class. It seems undeniably true that more time is devoted to teaching
than to testing. Therefore, a common injunction is that tests are likely to check no
more than a limited portion of what has been taught. As a result, there should be a
degree of similarity between the texts used in class and those presented on tests.
They should be similar but also different enough so as to test what learners are
able to do and not simply what they can remember. The major difficulty at this
point resides in determining which texts are more suitable and reflect the same
level of difficulty of the texts used in class.
51
The fourth difference is that tests have consequences (Alderson, 1996 apud Nuttall,
1996:214). Unlike texts used for teaching purposes, texts as testing devices are
expected to measure the candidate’s level of knowledge and as a result determine
the candidate’s success or failure. Alderson (op. cit., 1996) defines tests as nerve-
wracking experiences in the sense that they comprise consequences which ordinary
teaching activities do not have. Students can pass or fail and this fact places
considerable pressure on those examinees who may only have a single chance to
achieve their aims, such as entering university, receiving a promotion, passing to
the next level at school, etc. As a consequence, feelings such as stress and
anxiety are generated since the students are being judged according to their
performance on a particular occasion. This amount of pressure experienced by
candidates/learners is often likely to lead to low performance.
Alderson and Wall (1993) seem to agree that tests appear to influence the way
teachers conduct lessons and the way learners learn. As far as the validation of
language testing is concerned, it seems important to highlight the relevance of the
Washback Hypothesis and Impact Studies in the sense that they appear to bring
pedagogical implications to the teaching and/or learning of languages. These
issues will be addressed in the next subsection.
2.2.5. The relevance of “washback” and “impact” research
...our classroom observation research suggests that test preparation does
not have to imply “teaching for the test” in a narrow way. We saw
teachers being both creative and effective by setting out to teach the
skills that are tested, rather than concentrating too narrowly on practice
test questions. (Green and Hawkey, 2004:69)
52
Within the language testing scenario, the Washback Hypothesis emerges as a
central and complex issue (Alderson & Wall, 1993 apud Green and Hawkey).
Washback is often addressed as the extent to which testing is likely to affect
teaching and learning (Alderson & Wall, 1993 apud Green and Hawkey). It is
claimed to have both negative and positive effects. In an attempt to clarify the
distinction between the two types of Washback, Green and Hawkey (2004:66)
highlight that Washback may be negative when teaching and learning are restricted
to passing a given test. As to positive Washback, the same authors argue that well-
designed tests with clear and well-defined objective are claimed to lead to positive
Washback. Furthermore, the authors suggest that determining the nature of the
Washback of a particular test is often difficult since there is a number of factors
involved such as teachers, learners and classroom materials.
Alderson and Wall (1993, apud Green and Hawkey, 2004:67) highlight the
importance of research conducted in the area of testing In order to probe the
Washback Hypothesis. Results have revealed that the way Language teachers
conduct their lessons is claimed to have significant influence on the Washback of
language tests. By replicating the kinds of tasks found in tests and focusing on the
development of specific skills so as to enable their learners to achieve high scores,
teachers corroborate this assumption. However, the influence of tests does not
seem to be restricted to the language classroom (Taylor, 2004 apud Green and
Hawkey, 2004:68). Tests are also likely to influence decisions about curriculum
planning, immigration policy, or professional registration for doctors. This feature of
tests is often called Test Impact, i.e., the wider influences of tests beyond classroom
boundaries.
53
Due to the apparent relevance of tests as evidence of performance which is likely
to promote changes in the lives of individuals or groups, the providers of
international examinations such as the FCE have been more concerned with
another crucial aspect of testing: ethics. Shohamy (1999, apud Green and
Hawkey, 2004:68), who advocates a critical view of testing, argues that it seems
risky to regard tests as powerful in the sense that they lead to momentous decisions
affecting individuals and programs. This view suggests that language tests with big
impact ought to have properly-defined targets, appropriate and reliable evaluation
criteria, transparent and fair test interpretation and reporting systems, continuous
validation processes, and a keen regard for the rights of candidates and other stakeholders
(Green and Hawkey, 2004:69).
2.2.6. Learning validity: a concern in language testing
Seville (2003, apud Green and Hawkey, 2004:69) advocates that effective
language testing needs to pursue test validation. This view seems to suggest that:
a) tests are expected to reflect the purpose(s) for which they have been devised;
b) they should present similar results after repeated uses; c) they should reveal a
positive Washback and Impact. Tomlinson (2005:39) claims that apart from being
fair, valid and reliable, the ultimate aim should be providing learners and
professionals in charge of administering the tests with real learning opportunities
(i.e.: learning validity). Learners can learn from being tested (Tomlinson, 2005:39).
From his perspective, learners might benefit from their preparation, from taking tests
and from constructive feedback whilst and after taking them (Tomlinson, 2005:39).
54
However, if on one hand learning validity emerges as a crucial element in
language testing, on the other hand, developing useful language tests is often
regarded as a source of concern among researchers (Backman & Palmer,
1997:129). The plan for evaluating the qualities of usefulness, according to
Backman & Palmer (1997:131), is expected to comprise two main aspects in test
development: a) the areas of language ability to be measured and b) the
resources available for developing the test. The former seems to comprise aspects
of validity whereas the latter contemplates aspects of practicality. Backman &
Palmer (1997:129) suggest that the process of test development should be based
on a set of pre-established criteria which, by the same token, is expected to
include a set of descriptions and definitions. First and foremost, test designers
should start by identifying the purpose of the test to be designed (i.e., inferences
about language ability). Second, they are expected to identify the target discourse
community of the tests which seems to comprise test takers, teachers and
examiners/supervisors. As far as the characteristics of test takers are concerned,
Backman & Palmer (1997:129) argue that there are four main aspects to be
considered: a) personal characteristics; b) topical knowledge; c) general level and
profile of language ability and d) prediction about test takers potential affective
responses to test. Third, the program in which the test will be inserted should also
be a source of concern. All these elements should be carefully considered in order
to devise suitable tasks.
As far as learning validity in the testing of reading is concerned, research studies
conducted in the area of genre analysis might be regarded as an approachable
alternative in order to promote positive Washback and Impact. In the next section,
55
the relevance of genre studies will be addressed. Similarly, there will be an
attempt to reflect on their applications in order to develop more effective reading
lessons.
2.3. REVISITING GENRE THEORIES
2.3.1. Background to Text Linguistics
The oldest form of preoccupation with texts can be found in RHETORIC,
dating from Ancient Greece and Rome through the Middle Ages right up
to the present resurgence of classical rhetoric. (Beaugrande and
Dressler, 1981:15)
As the citation above suggests, the investigation of both spoken and written texts
have always constituted a source of concern among theoreticians. However, even
though the study of ”Rhetoric” dates back to ancient times, it still shares a number
of concerns related to a particular field of Modern Applied Linguistics, namely Text
Linguistics. According to Koch (2003:07), Text Linguistics emerged in Europe in the
1960s where it gained fame in the 1970s. At that time, its major focal point was
the investigation and description of syntactic-semantic relationships both at the
level of the sentence and stretches of sentences within a given paragraph.
However, in the 1970s, there was a substantial shift in focus. If, on one hand
researchers were still tied to the concepts of structural grammar, on the other
hand there was an apparent increase in interest towards developing what would
become known as Textual Grammar (Koch, 2003:07). In other words, researchers
engaged in reflecting upon aspects such as textual cohesion and coherence. As a
56
result, analyses were no longer restricted to the level of the sentence, since
Textual Linguistics had shifted its focus to the description of the linguistic
phenomena which could not be explained through the often-held structural
grammar perspective.
Beaugrande & Dressler (1981:3) define Text Linguistics as being the science of text
whose major concern is to describe and explain features texts have in common,
on the one hand, and to determine the existing differences among them, on the
other. Furthermore, Text Linguistics should be able to determine the standards to
which texts should conform as well as how these same texts were produced and
received. In addition, it should also focus on the reasons why readers would
tackle particular texts within a given discourse community.
A lingüística textual trata o texto como um ato de comunicação
unificado num complexo universo de ações humanas
13
. (Marcuschi,
1983, apud Koch, 2003:11)
It is important to bear in mind that the investigations developed in the field of Text
Linguistics are usually based on the notion whereby a text does not consist of a
sequence of isolated sentences, but a linguistic unit which presents peculiar structural
features (Koch, 2003:07). From this perspective, texts seem to constitute vehicles
to promote meaningful human interactions (Beaugrande & Dressler, 1981:3). This
position has also been strengthened by Teun Van Dijk (1979, apud Beaugrande e
Dressler, 1981:14) who pointed out that Text Linguistics does not consist of a
designation for a single theory or method. Instead, it designates any work in language
13
Text Linguistics contemplates texts as communicative occurrences within a complex universe of
human actions. [my translation]
57
science devoted to the text as the primary object of inquiry. Marcuschi (1983, apud
Koch, 2003:10) supports this view and goes even further by highlighting that Text
Linguistics is a field of study which investigates both the linguistic and cognitive
operations which govern the production, construction and understanding of texts.
In the author’s view, Text Linguistics acknowledges the crucial importance of both
linear (i.e.: the value of existing grammatical dependencies as major signals
towards constructing meanings) and non-linear organization (i.e.: graphic
conventions, reference apparatus and figures). Similarly, it investigates surface
cohesion (i.e.: linguistic constituents), conceptual coherence (i.e.: at the semantic
and cognitive levels) and pragmatic functions.
Interestingly, despite the advances in the area of Text Linguistics, Silva (2004:53)
asserts that a substantial number of the current views on texts still often seem to
be restricted to a list of pre-established formulas to which written texts should
conform, regardless of both the communicative intent, and the social context,
within which these same texts are inserted. In the 1980s, Modern Text Linguistics
promoted the development of a number of theories which generated a diversity of
branches further outlined in Koch (2003:8-10), and including:
Beaugrande & Dressler (1981) – These authors have placed particular
emphasis on the investigation of the seven major patterns of textuality (i.e.:
cohesion, coherence, intentionality, acceptability, situationality, intertextuality and
informativity). They also have the cognitive processing of texts as a focal
point. Beaugrande & Dressler (op. cit.) advocate that the first two patterns
of textuality listed above (i.e.: cohesion and coherence) are text-bound
58
whereas the other five are reader-bound. As to the cognitive processing of
texts, the authors highlight the crucial role played by both the readers’ socially-
acquired knowledge and the propositional/declarative meanings towards
promoting proficient reading.
Weinrich – This author’s research study focuses on the development of a
discourse macrosintax whereby written texts are tackled as linear
elements.
Givón – This author is concerned with linguistic construction of meanings at
the sentence level on one hand and with cognitive processing (i.e.:
production/comprehension processes) of texts on the other. He is
particularly interested in the investigation of the cognitive mechanisms and
models involved in act of processing texts.
Petöfi – This author was engaged in the development of a semiotic theory
of oral texts, namely TeSWeST (i.e.: Text Structure Theory – World
Structure Theory). He aimed at answering questions concerned with textual
comprehension and production by establishing a relation between the
internal structure of a given text (i.e.: the co-textual features) and its
possible interpretations (i.e.: contextual features).
Van Dijk – This author has focused on the study of textual macropatterns
and typology of texts. He has focused on the field of Critical Discourse
Analysis since 1985.
59
Schmidt – This author describes textuality as any sort of communication,
realized through linguistic symbols or not.
A shift in focus in Text Linguistics, in Hoey’s (1997) views, has led to pedagogical
implications related to the teaching of reading, as far as reading strategies are
concerned. Hoey (op. cit.) argues that Text Linguistics perceives written texts as
semantic entities (Halliday & Hasan, 1976 apud Hoey 1997) i.e., texts derive their
coherence from being meaningful (Hoey, 1997). Thus, according to Hoey (op. cit.),
rather than restricting reading lessons to the identification of the cognitive
processes undergone by readers when approaching texts, teachers of reading
might be advised to focus upon the interactive nature of texts. In the author’s view,
the development of meaning-centered text analyses may prove to be an effective
means towards raising learners’ awareness regarding the interactive properties of
texts as semantic entities (Hoey, op. cit.). In this same author’s view, there is a wide
range of benefits in focusing on reading from the perspective of text analysis: Since
reading is about the derivation of meaning from a text, it is not unreasonable that a
meaning-centered analysis might be helpful in the teaching of reading (Hoey, 1997).
Among such key advantages, Hoey (1997) suggests that text analysis a) seems to
encourage natural reading, b) engages learners in gradually making sense of the
target text and c) provides learners with intensive and memorable experience of the
vocabulary items which are essential to build up the texture (Hoey, 1997:10) of
target texts.
60
2.3.2. Approaching reading via text analysis
In fact with the help of text analysis you can turn your learners into
confident and independent explorers of texts. Who knows what
discoveries they will make? (Hoey, 1997:17)
It is a commonplace among theoreticians to argue that reading consists of an
interactive process in which meaning is central. It may be thus inferred that
meaning-centered text analysis may be beneficial to the development of effective
reading strategies among FL learners in general. Furthermore, text analysis may
contribute significantly to a more thorough understanding of what effective reading
entails in a broader sense.
According to Hoey (1997:03), there are three major distinctive textual features
which teachers should bear in mind when planning their L1 or FL reading lessons
i.e., texts are interactive, cohesive and patterned. In this author’s view, a thorough
understanding of such features may provide a valuable contribution to the
development of effective reading classes. Thus, due to the purposes of this
investigation and the corpus to be examined, an overview of the three textual
features described by Hoey (op. cit.) will be provided in the following subsections.
2.3.2.1. The interactive nature of reading
The first textual feature cited by Hoey (1997) is that texts are interactive pieces of
language. In the author’s view, texts might well be described in metaphorical terms
61
as unvoiced dialogues or a waltz between writer and reader. From this perspective,
writers attempt to determine the information the reader wishes to obtain, whereas,
readers, by the same token, attempt to guess which data is likely to be provided by
the writer within the flow of the discourse:
The reader’s guess will be based partly upon the knowledge that the
writer will try and answer the reader’s questions. The writer’s guess on
the other hand will be based partly upon the knowledge that the reader
will try and second-guess what the writer is going to say. (Hoey, 1997)
Conversely, whether the interaction between writer and reader will occur as
idealized in the first place is often difficult to determine. However, if the dance
develops smoothly, the reading process may be either effortless or substantially
easier (Hoey, 1997). Skillful readers are often able to identify the clues spread
over the surface of the text. They are aware of the fact that writers may resort to a
wide range of mechanisms in their efforts to convey the intended message (i.e.,
the answers to the readers’ questions):
These markers of the questions that a writer thinks important are
obviously of interest to any reader, in that they can be seen as intended to
guide the reader into a certain interpretation. (Hoey, 1997:11)
According to Hoey (op.cit.), writers may signal the relevance of the questions they
intend to answer in several different ways such as through the use of:
a) rhetorical questions: These questions are provided explicitly and followed by
their corresponding answers, which are often lengthy.
62
b) Embedded questions: the first half of these questions provides the question itself,
whereas the second half provides the writer’s answer – such as in the
example: What language learners need is more effective reading strategies (i.e.,
question: What do language learners need? answer more reading
strategies).
c) Lexis of questions (e.g.: question, answer, explanation etc). Sentences such as The
answer to this problem is a very simple one invites the reader to continue reading
in order to identify the solution to the problem.
In Hoey’s (op. cit.) view this process of predicting what writers are likely to say
next should be cultivated in the reading classroom. According to the author, such
procedures may prove to be highly beneficial for learners. Thus, by engaging in
the process of predicting what comes next, learners are likely to expand their
awareness of the processes they undergo when reading in their L1, and, therefore,
be even more likely to transfer any related skills to the target language.
Furthermore, by being predictive (Hoey, op. cit.) learners work out the answers to
their inquiries by themselves, undoubtedly one step towards becoming more
independent. However, if on the one hand, the benefits of this approach to reading
are clearly evident, they also suggest a substantial amount of preparation on the
teachers’ part, on the other. Furthermore, the benefits presuppose a certain
degree of familiarity with a diversity of textual genres: the development of appropriate
predictive skills is a good a way as any of teaching the informational properties of
particular genres (Hoey, 1997:20). The issues surrounding the development of a
63
genre-oriented approach to reading will be discussed in depth further in the
present section.
2.3.2.2. Texts are cohesive
Cohesion is an overt feature of the text, providing surface evidence for
the text’s unity and connectedness. (Celce-Murcia & Olshstain, E.,
2000:126)
The second distinctive feature of text is that text is cohesive (Halliday & Hasan,
1976:07 and Hoey, 1997). Cohesion may be realized linguistically through the use
of either lexical or grammatical cohesive mechanisms (Koch, 2003:16), which are
heavily based on the reader’s linguistic competence. According to Beaugrande &
and Dressler (1981:50), such mechanisms establish semantic relations among
closely-knit units, spread along the text surface, in order to promote the construction
of meanings at different levels (i.e.: phrases, clauses or sentences):
É por meio de mecanismos como estes que se vai tecendo o “tecido”
(tessitura) do texto. A este fenômeno é que se denomina coesão textual
14
.
(Koch, 2003:15)
Hoey (1997) asserts that the identification of cohesive features spread along the
text surface may prove to be of crucial importance in expanding reading strategies.
The same author argues that such procedure strengths the notion of texture i.e.,
texts are made up of sentences in combination (Widdowson, 1973 apud Hoey,1995:10)
14
It is through the use of mechanisms like these that the texture of the target text is built. This
phenomenon is called textual cohesion. [my translation]
64
and corroborates the view that even though written discourse is surely secondary
to spoken discourse, it is not inferior as far as the level of complexity is concerned
(Shepherd, 1984:145):
... a língua tem dois modos, a fala e a escrita, que são variáveis,
paralelos mas não permutáveis meios de expressão contrastivos
15
.
(Shepherd, 1984: 146)
Celce-Murcia & Olshstain (2000:126) highlight the importance of being able to
identify the cohesive chains (Halliday and Hasan, 1976) within a text. They argue
that such chains are used by writers in order to facilitate reader interpretative
processes, aimed at achieving the communicative intent. In Hoey’s (op. cit.) view,
there are advantages in focusing on the identification of the cohesive ties spread
along the surface of texts. Among such benefits, the author highlights that spotting
cohesive links requires little or no specialist knowledge on the learner’s part.
Furthermore, apart from developing learners’ reading strategies, the tasks devised
to identify cohesive features in texts may prove to be fun, challenging,
encouraging and enhance learners’ sense of achievement. Conversely, low
linguistic competence on the reader’s part may lead to difficulties throughout the
interpretation process (Celce-Murcia M. & Olshstain, E., 2000:126).
... um estudo mais minuciosos dos mecanismos, do “input lingüístico e
das funções lingüísticas pertinentes à escrita poderia perfeitamente
fornecer detalhes relevantes ao ensino da leitura, também
16
. (Shepherd,
1984:146)
15
A given language may be expressed in two different forms: spoken and written. Both forms
although changeable, are contrastive means of expression. [my translation]
16
A more detailed investigation of the mechanisms, the linguistic input and linguistic functions
relevant to writing shall reveal important findings to the teaching of reading. [my translation]
65
2.3.2.3. Texts are patterned
As Lopes (1999:76) avers, a number of linguists and genre analysts seem to
agree that the communicative purposes which govern genres impose constraints
on both content and form. Hoey (1997:21) strengthens this view by arguing that
texts are patterned. According to this author, patterns consist of sets of questions
which both writer and reader expect to be answered within the flow of the
discourse. Such questions are signaled to the reader in a variety of ways,
depending on their degree of relevance. In addition, the patterns may occur in
paragraphs as well as in larger pieces of texts.
Using the signals of the patterns is a bit like following the tourist
signposts pointing you to all the main sights of a place. The places that
are signposted are usually worth seeing. (Hoey, 1997)
Another factor particularly relevant in order to achieve a better understanding of
how writers tend to organize, and readers tend to process, discourse is the
concept of clause relation. Hoey (1986:190) claims that all patterns are composed of
clause relations which, by the same token, may be seen as abstractions from the
questions a writer seeks to answer and a reader sees being answered, in a discourse
(Hoey, 1986:189). From this clause-relational perspective, it may be inferred that
whenever readers approach pieces of language that apparently belong together,
they are likely to relate them by either matching or sequencing them. Such a
processing strategy, according to Hoey (1986:189) is independent of the language in
which the information is expressed.
66
A further aspect to be considered is that, by attending to the clues spread along
the text surface, readers are likely to reach more precise interpretations. The
nature of organizational patterns lies in the sense of order perceived by a reader,
i.e., how clauses and groups of clauses relate to each other (e.g.: expressing
contrast relations, generalization-example relations, etc.). Thus, describing a
particular organizational pattern consists of describing the relations of which it is the
outcome (Hoey, 1986:190). The methods used in order to identify the patterns may
vary. However, they all should be concerned with conveying the meaning(s)
identified by readers as a result of the placement of the sentences.
As far as the analysis of genre-texts is concerned, Hoey (1997:22) asserts that
text analysis may be conducted in two distinct ways depending on its ultimate
goal: by considering the genre-specific patterns (Swales, 1990 apud Hoey, 1997) or
the genre-general patterns (Winter, 1977 apud Hoey, 1997) of target texts. Thus,
given the specific purposes of the present research, it is of crucial importance to
have a clear understanding of both kinds of patterns. For this reason, a more
detailed discussion on both the genre-specific patterns and the genre-general
patterns is provided in the subsequent paragraphs. First and foremost, it is
important to bear in mind is that neither approach is more prominent than the
other. The criterion to be adopted simply reflects the purposes of the analysis to
be developed.
67
2.3.3. The genre-specific patterns
Bhatia (1993) and Swales (1990) also argue that a particular communicative event
which displays most of the conventionalized characteristics of a particular genre
constitutes a prototypical exemplar of such genre. According to these authors,
genres are realized through specific moves and patterns which unveil the
organizational possibilities of the exemplars. From this perspective, the language
organization functions as a vehicle to achieve the writer’s communicative intent,
i.e., the organizational possibilities of the genres are governed by the
communicative purposes of the discourse communities in which such genres are
inserted.
Many linguists, including Swales (1990), Bhatia (1993) and Motta-Roth (1995)
have investigated particular genres in order to find elements in common as far
their internal organization is concerned. Bhatia (1993), for instance, has
elaborated a Promotional Genre Framework which provides the patterns for
analyzing two specific genres: promotional letters and job applications. These
patterns are outlined in Tables 2.2 and 2.3 below:
1. Establishing credentials;
2. Introducing the offer;
3. Offering incentives;
4. Enclosing documents;
5. Soliciting response;
6. Using pressure tactics;
7. Endin
g
p
olitel
y
.
Table 2.2.: Bhatia’s Promotional Genre Framework for analyzing sales promotion letters.
68
Similarly, Motta-Roth (1995) developed a framework for the identification of the
rhetorical moves in academic book reviews based on Swales´ (1990) analyses.
The former author’s schematic description of the rhetorical moves of this genre is
further presented in Table 2.4 below:
Swales (1990), on the other hand, has identified the CARS (Create a Research
Space) model for research article introductions:
1. Establishing credentials;
2. Introducing candidature;
3. Offering incentives;
4. Enclosing documents;
5. Soliciting response;
6. Using pressure tactics;
7. Ending politely.
Move 1 – Introducing the book
Sub-function 1: defining the general topic of the book and/or
Sub-function 2: informing about potential readership and/or
Sub-function 3: informing about the author and/or
Sub-function 4: making topic generalizations and/or
Sub-function 5: inserting book in the field
Move 2 – Outlining the book
Sub-function 6: providing general view of the organization of the book and/or
Sub-function 7: stating the topic of each chapter and/or
Sub-function 8: citing extra-text material
Move 3 – Highlighting parts of the book
Sub-function 9: providing focused evaluation
Table 2.4: Motta-Roth´s (1995) Academic Book Review Moves
Table 2.3.: Bhatia’s Promotional Genre Framework for analyzing job application letters.
69
Further research studies conducted in the field of genre analysis have revealed
that the models presented in the previous pages account for specific genre-texts,
namely promotional letters, job applications, academic book reviews and
research article introductions. However, due to the wide range of genre-texts
which constitute the corpus of this investigation, it may be inferred that the models
previously outlined would not be suitable for the identification of all genre-texts to
be examined. For this reason, the adoption of an alternative framework seems
advisable, in view of the variety of the genre-texts encountered.
Move 1: Establishing a territory
Step 1: Claiming centrality
and/or
Step 2: Making topic generalization(s)
and/or
Step 3: Reviewing items of previous research
Move 2: Establishing a niche
Step 1
A
Counter-claiming
or
Step 1B Indicating a gap
Step 1C Question-raising
or
Step 1D Continuing a tradition
Move 3 Occupying a niche
Step 1
A
Outlining purposes
or
Step 1B: Announcing present research
Step 2 Announcing principal findings
Step 3 Indicating RA structure.
Table 2.5: Swales (1990) CARS Model for article introduction
s
(
adapted from Swales 1990, fi
g
ure 10, p. 141
).
70
2.3.4. The genre-general patterns
Hoey (1997) also describes internal genre-text organization in terms of patterns,
which reveal the needs and goals of the writer. However, this author follows Winter
(1977) and prefers to describe texts in terms of genre-general patterns. He defines
the genre-general patterns as sets of recurrent questions that writers and readers
agree should be asked and answered (Hoey, 1997). Hoey (1986:191) argues that
there is a wide range of methods in order to identify genre-patterns. The most
basic way consisting of projecting the discourse into a question-and-answer
dialog. According to Hoey (op. cit.), there is a diversity of questions which may
unveil the relationship between the pairs of sentences.
Furthermore, Hoey (1997) asserts that in western society texts contain patterns
which occur and reoccur marking similar organization in a variety of texts.
Therefore, as a means of illustrating the diversity of existing patterns, Hoey (1997)
highlights the most recurrent questions which tend to be answered through textual
organization. In order to illustrate the diversity of existing patterns in European
languages, three of these groups have been summarized in the form of questions
in Table 2.6. below:
Table 2.6: Genre-general patterns (Adapted from Hoey, 1997)
GENRE-GENERAL PATTERNS
PROBLEM SOLUTION
PATTERN
QUESTION-ANSWER
PATTERN
GOAL-ACHIEVEMENT
PATTERN
What was the problem? What do we need to know? What did we want to
achieve?
What did someone do
about it?
How did we go about
finding out?
How did we go about doing
this?
SAMPLE
QUESTIONS
How successful was
this?
How successful was this? How successful were we?
71
Hoey (1997) argues that the sets of questions presented above may account for
both simple and complex texts. Furthermore, since the genre-general patterns of
written discourse organization consist of sets of global questions which both
reader and writer expect to be answered, writers like to signal them, to ensure that the
reader recognizes that such questions are being answered (Hoey, 1997). Hence, key
lexical items, which are typical signals of a particular pattern, are utilized. Thus, in
the problem-solution pattern, for example, it is common to encounter words such
as difficulty, problem, shortcoming, solution etc. In the goal-achievement pattern, on
the other hand, it would be common to find words such as objective, success and
intention.
However, if each genre-general pattern seems to reveal particular pattern-
signaling vocabulary items (i.e.: key lexical items which draw the readers’ attention
to the questions being answered by the writer), Hoey (1997) claims that
possessing an extensive vocabulary constitutes no guarantee that meanings will
be constructed accordingly. This seems to suggest that even though key lexical
items may be regarded as clues spread along the text surface (Hoey, 1997),
teachers of reading in general possibly ought to be more concerned about
equipping learners with a wider range of strategies in order to enable them to
approach texts in a variety of ways according to their needs and aims. This notion
corroborates the view that reading effectively does not mean sucking up every word
and nuance of meaning in the text (Hoey, 1997). In longer texts, for instance, more
than one pattern may be identified by readers, depending on how well the text and
the discourse has been organized by the respective writer:
So the property of text that it is patterned can directly help the reader read
more effectively. All the reader has to be able to do is recognize the
72
characteristic signals of the patterns (...), and they are useful in writing and the
spoken skills as well. (Hoey, 1997)
2.3.5. Defining Genre: Swales’ (1990) Working Definition of Genre
The focal point of the current investigation, as highlighted in the introductory
section, is to develop a genre-based analysis of the reading comprehension
questions proposed to readers both in the FCE reading paper and in the reading
sections of coursebooks by different publishers. Furthermore, it is intended to both
determine the textual genre(s) which are predominant in both sources, as well as,
define the target discourse community of the texts and their corresponding
comprehension questions. Finally, an attempt will be made to reflect upon the
extent to which our findings may be revealing for TEFL, more specifically for the
Brazilian private EFL teaching/learning scenario, where learners are often trained
for international examinations.
Swales’ working definition of genre (1990:58) has been selected as a theoretical
basis for our investigation. The decision to adopt this author’s definition may be
justified by its relevance to the field of genre analysis. Swales’ definition of genre is a
synthesis of contemporary interpretations of the term (...). It provides us with a way of
looking at language in use which differs in many respects from that inherent in process
approaches, but which can complement them (Tribble, 1996:23). Moreover, Swales’
definition has been widely used as a valuable starting point for the development of
a diversity of investigations in the field of genre analysis. Swales (op. cit.) defines
a genre as:
73
... a class of communicative events, the members of which share
some set of communicative purposes. These purposes are
recognized by the expert members of the parent discourse
community, and thereby constitute the rationale for the genre.
This rationale shapes the schematic structure of the discourse
and influences and constrains choice of content and style.
Communicative purpose is both a privileged criterion and one
that operates to keep the scope of a genre as here conceived
narrowly focused on comparable rhetorical action. In addition
to purpose, exemplars of a genre exhibit various patterns of
similarity in terms of structure, style, content and intended
audience. If all high probability expectations are realized, the
exemplar will be viewed as prototypical by the parent discourse
community. The genre names inherited and produced by
discourse communities are imported by others constitute
valuable ethnographic communication, but typically need
further validation. (Swales, 1990: 58)
Thus, in Swales´ view, a genre consists of a class of communicative events which are
determined by specific communicative purposes. The former comprises the
discourse itself, the participants involved and the environment in which the
discourse is produced. The latter is related to the role of the discourse i.e., using
language to get something done. As Tribble (1996:23) asserts, language exists to
fulfill certain functions. Communicative purposes are, therefore, manifested
linguistically by means of lexical items and grammatical structures.
Focusing on texts from a genre-oriented perspective, according to Swales (op.
cit.), would seem to involve the application of various categories for the analysis of
written text or discourse, in order to determine those elements which are common
to the internal organization i.e., the generic structure of the discourse (Tribble,
1996:32). Bhatia (1993:16) subscribes to Swales’ (ibid) views when he argues that
each genre is an instance of a successful achievement of a specific communicative purpose
using conventionalized knowledge of linguistic and discoursive resources. Within this
74
scope, the notion of communicative purpose emerges as an essential element in
order to investigate the nature of texts. Halliday & Hasan (1985/1989), for
instance, argue that a text may be defined as a functional use of language. By
functional it is meant that language realizes a particular function within a given
context, in opposition to the idea of text as isolated words or sentences. It is
certainly true that texts may share similar features, including status, lexis and
typography. However, Swales (op. cit.) argues that it is the communicative purpose
of a given genre that is more important than any other formal feature (Tribble,
1996: 34).
A further central factor to be considered in the identification of a genre is related to
the organization of information; which is based on functions, in contrast to an
analysis of a “register”, which would present, as a primary focus, the lexical or
syntactic choices made by writers. As Droga & Humphrey (2002:4) claim, genres
occur in order to achieve a goal or purpose. In order to achieve its purpose a genre moves
through distinctive stages, with beginnings, middles and ends which are recognizable.
Such stages are defined by Tribble (1996:32) as predictable. From this perspective,
any genre would be considered a communicative event materialized via spoken or
written language aimed at a particular discourse community in order to achieve a
given communicative intent.
2.3.6. The social role of genres
In view of the issues discussed above, it seems undeniably true that genres
possess a remarkable role within social interactions. This is the notion which
75
appears to be particularly relevant to the development of effective L1 and/or FL
reading lessons. By focusing on genres, teachers bring society, as an integrating
element, into and as part of the language classroom (Gazotti, 1999:14). This view
corroborates the assumption whereby a text consists of a product of the categories of
social interactions that are realized by genres (Tribble, 1996:26). Bakhtin (op. cit.)
highlights this characteristic feature of genres by stating that human beings often
create rules to be followed in order to interact according to the environment in
which they are inserted. Such rules reflect the social behavior which is manifested
through linguistic choices. A genre would be a set of rules defined by the members of a
determined social context to be followed as patterns when producing the language in these
contexts (Gazotti, 1999:14).
Thus, as the preceding paragraph suggests, the act of writing within a genre does
not seem to prioritize a straightforward focus on form (Tribble, 1996:32). This notion
seems to be particularly related to Swales’ concept of schematic structure which
suggests that even if writers do not possess absolute control of a given language
system, they are likely to communicate successfully within the scope of a
particular genre. By arguing in this way, Swales (op.cit.) brings the notion of
“formal schemata” into the arena of genre research studies. The author claims that
“formal schemata” enables writers to produce appropriately organized texts
according to their ultimate aim. In addition, it is a combination of procedural, formal
and content schemata which may enable readers to identify particular genres, as
well as evaluate their degree of prototypicality i.e., to what extent such texts may
be considered a true representative of a set of texts.
76
Bakhtin (1929) also highlighted the crucial role played by the social environment
within the manifestation of linguistic phenomena. This author used the expression
linguistic community whereas Swales (1990), preferred to address the same
phenomenon as discourse community, therefore corroborating the relevance of the
social context for the production and comprehension of a given language. In
Bakhtin’s (op.cit.) view, it is fundamental that both the encoder and the decoder
belong to the same linguistic community i.e., an organized society. In addition, it is
fundamental that the two individuals are integrated within the unity of the
immediate social situation. Marcuschi (2002), by the same token, argues that
textual genres consist of historical phenomena strongly linked to an individual´s
cultural and social life. Thus, they derive from particular needs linked to the social
and cultural life of a given group of individuals i.e., (Swales, op.cit.): discourse
community:
(Os gêneros) surgem, situam-se e integram-se funcionalmente nas
culturas em que se desenvolvem
17
. (Marcuschi, 2002)
Thus, rather than being governed by linguistic and structural features, genres are
largely characterized by their communicative function. Marcuschi (op. cit.)
subscribes to Swales’ views when he argues that genres constitute socio-
discoursive entities which govern our daily-life activities. Hence, genres are not
static, in the sense that they do not limit human creativity. As Bakhtin (op.cit.),
asserts, the unlimited number of human activities result in an equally diverse
amount of textual genres. From this perspective, Marcuschi (2002) defines textual
genres as being dynamic and plastic:
17
Genres emerge and are functionally integrated in the cultures where they are developed. [my
translation]
77
[Muitos gêneros] são de difícil definição formal, devendo ser
contemplados em seus usos e condicionamentos sócio-pragmáticos
caracterizados como práticas sócio-discursivas. Quase inúmeros em
diversidade de formas, obtêm denominações nem sempre unívocas...
18
.
(Marcuschi, 2002)
It might be inferred that any new genre which emerges from technological
innovations, for instance, should not be regarded as an absolute innovation in the
sense that it results from already pre-existing genres. The “new” genres appear to
establish new relationships with the uses of the language. They create new forms
of communication characterized by their hybrid (Fairclough, 1989) nature.
A preliminary conclusion which may be drawn, in very general terms, is that textual
genres cannot be regarded as well-defined, static structures (i.e., textual families
grouped together according to their similarities). In fact, textual genres constitute
“linguistic events” which should not be defined by their linguistic characteristics but
by their socio-discoursive roles. They constitute culture-sensitive socio-historical
phenomena and reflect the social structures which are typical in each and every
culture (Malinowski, 1926). Thus, in order to build a text efficiently so as to achieve
the communicative intent, Swales (1990) argues that writers should bear in mind
two crucial aspects, namely (a) the Context of Culture and (b) the Context of
Situation.
18
A wide range of genres are not easily defined. Thus, due to the diversity of forms which may be
encountered, genres should be contemplated according to their socio-pragmatic functions. They
constitute socio-discoursive practices whose names do not usually emerge from common
agreement. [my translation]
78
2.3.7. The Context of Culture
As discussed in preceding pages, Swales’ (op. cit.) view of the concept of genre
embodies the identification of discursive patterns. These patterns should be
recognized by a given discourse community within a particular communicative
event. From this perspective, texts should present similarities in content, style,
target audience and communicative purposes. If and when all these features are
fulfilled, then a given group of texts is said to be representative of a particular
genre. Thus, it might be correct to infer that individuals who share the same goals
tend to develop particular kinds of genre, either oral or written. Consequently, the
knowledge of the Context of Culture enables writers to shape meanings
accordingly in order to achieve the communicative intent they have in mind. In
order to achieve such aim, writers count on their familiarity with the discoursive
patterns which govern a particular discourse community.
2.3.8. The Context of Situation
In contrast to the previous definitions of the Context of Culture, the Context of
Situation from Swales’ (op. cit.) perspective seems to be directly related to the
rhetorical moves i.e., the linguistic choices made by writers. Such choices are also
defined as “register”. Halliday and Hasan (1976:22) assert that the linguistic features
which are typically associated with a configuration of situational features – with particular
values of the field, mode and tenor
19
– constitute a register. In other words, the register
19
Halliday & Hasan (op. cit.) argue that field, tenor and mode refer to the text itself, the relation
established between the participants (i.e.: writer and reader) and the vehicle through which the
discourse is expressed (i.e.: oral or spoken text), respectively.
79
appears to emerge from a combination of the two dimensions of a given text i.e.,
its Context of Culture and its linguistic manifestation (i.e., the Context of Situation).
Thus, if we bear in mind that the Context of Situation determines the linguistic
choices realized by the authors in order to achieve their communicative intent, it
may be concluded that a detailed investigation of the Context of Situation might
reveal how language may be used as a tool to shape meanings through a given
genre (Droga & Humphrey, 2002:02). According to Halliday and Hasan (op. cit.),
Field, tenor and mode are inserted in the Context of Situation and constitute the
three major dimensions of the discourse:
Field – It is related to the nature of the social activity and its purpose. Its
semantic realization is related to experiential meanings.
Tenor – It is related to the roles and relationships between the interactants.
Its semantic realization conveys interpersonal meanings.
Mode – It is related to the channel or medium of communication – whether
the text is spoken or written is likely to determine action and reflection. Its
semantic realization expresses textual meanings i.e, using language in a
coherent and meaningful way.
These three aspects within a functional model are claimed to influence the shape
and realize meanings in a particular text (Droga & Humphrey,2002:21-30). As a
result, readers are able to predict aspects of the context of situation from specific
grammatical features and lexis.
80
2.4. Conclusions
In view of the discussion presented above and considering that all texts are likely
to be manifested through a given textual genre, it is important to highlight that a
more comprehensive knowledge of how textual genres function is a sine-qua-non
for effective understanding of texts. Paltridge (1996) and Dudley-Evans (1997:219)
highlight the relevance of these research studies developed in the field of textual
genre analysis to the teaching/learning of L2 or FL as well as for the
teaching/learning of ESP
20
:
(...) communicative purpose is, in fact, the defining feature by which a
genre such as the academic article is distinguished from other genres
and by which the consideration of genre is distinguished from the
consideration of register. (Dudley-Evans, 1997:219)
As the citation above suggests, the use of the term genre to describe those
studies developed in the area of ESP, EAP or in the field of Applied Linguistics,
appears to differ from its use in the area of literary studies in which a given genre
such as a comedy or a tragedy is often identified through its form. ... a genre is a
means of achieving a communicative goal that has evolved in response to particular
rhetorical needs and that a genre will change and evolve in response to changes in those
needs. The emphasis is thus on the means by which a text realizes its communicative
purpose rather than on establishing a system for the classification of genres (Dudley-
Evans, 1997:219).
20
Within the context of ESP teaching/learning, the term genre was first used by Tarone et al (1981)
via an article which highlighted the importance of both lexical and grammatical choices in order to
achieve a given communicative intent.
81
Gazotti (1999:7) asserts that the concept of genre has been widely applied to
language teaching, specifically for the teaching of the first language. In Australia,
England, Anglophile Canada, and in the French-speaking cantons of Switzerland,
for instance, a syllabus of a course of French as a mother tongue has focused on
genres as the major tool to teach the target language. As Gazotti (op.cit.) claims,
the concept of genre is not a new one. It is common practice in the language
classroom to invite learners to produce “business letters”, “invitations”, “letters of
complaint”, etc. By doing so, teachers are clearly working with genres. However,
teachers are so often concerned about teaching the structures of the target
language that they tend to overlook both the context in which the text was
produced, as well as the original purposes for which these texts were produced.
Gazotti exemplifies this major concern through the citation that follows:
In fact, the linguistic elements in a genre are a result of the choices a
community has made to create their texts. (...) that’s how grammar is
taught, within a specific context and being used for a specific purpose.
Gazotti (1999: 8)
The claims described in the preceding paragraphs arguably relate to practical
pedagogical implications for the teaching/testing of FL reading. The development
of committed pedagogical practice focused on genre analysis may help to
encourage learners to analyze and/or produce linguistic events, as well as identify
the major characteristics of a genre. Such tasks may also enhance learners’
reading skills, in addition to their capacity to produce their own texts. Therefore,
analyzing reading comprehension questions from a genre-based perspective
would involve, first and foremost, verifying whether the questions proposed to the
target audience contemplate the communicative nature of the target texts.
82
Furthermore, any analysis should also be able to determine to what extent the
comprehension questions call the learners’ attention to the linguistic choices which
govern the achievement of the communicative purposes of the texts involved.
In view of the arguments raised above, it may be possible to infer that reflective
pedagogical practice, including a focus on textual genres may be introduced within
the Brazilian private language schools sector. This would no doubt serve as a
valuable alternative, as it focuses, on the target language through its authentic
uses. This, in turn, may be achieved by a careful selection of appropriate
comprehension questions.
83
CHAPTER 3
METHODOLOGY
3.1. Selecting the corpus
The corpus of analysis selected for the present research study has been extracted
from two different sources. The first half consists of twenty reading comprehension
sections (i.e., texts and questions) from five publications widely used for the
teaching/learning of English either as a L2 or FL (Appendix 1). The coursebooks
are aimed at learners of English at the intermediate proficiency level and have
been published between the years 1996 and 2003. The decision to investigate the
coursebooks previously outlined was based on four parameters which considered
not only the texts and comprehension questions which accompany them but also
the reading skills required from learners when approaching the target texts:
a) Does each unit of the five coursebooks include a section on reading
comprehension?
b) Do the reading comprehension sections claim to relate specifically to the
FCE Reading Paper?
c) Do the texts and activities proposed claim to reflect the demands of the FCE,
as far as a genre-oriented approach to reading is concerned?
d) To what extent do the reading comprehension questions raise learners’
awareness regarding the generic organization of the texts and how does this
knowledge may be beneficial to them in their social practices?
21
21
...hoje a necessidade consiste em ensinar ao aluno a linguagem em uso na prática social; há uma
ênfase no “o que”, “para que” se está ensinando, no que isso vai ser importante na vida do
educando. (EDMUNDSON, 2004:88)
84
The second half of our corpus consists of four FCE reading papers (Appendix 2)
corresponding to the months June and December of the years 2003 and 2004.
Similarly, the texts and questions related to reading comprehension will be the
focus of analysis. The decision to investigate the previously described corpus (i.e.
the texts and questions related to reading comprehension) may be justified by the
recognition of the potential importance of the development of effective reading
within FL pedagogy, at least by international publishers, within the past two
decades. A further factor which has influenced this choice is the growing number
of people within the communities where I teach, who appear to be increasingly
interested in taking proficiency tests as a means to achieve more benefits,
specifically in professional settings. Clearly, the data to be investigated represents
no more than a tiny percentage within a realm of alternatives. However, the small
sample under analysis is believed to be acceptable for the purposes of the present
investigation in terms of their matching recent research regarding genre studies
and the development of reading skills in the field of foreign language learning.
3.2. Describing the corpus
3.2.1. The coursebooks
In this subsection, an attempt will be made to provide an objective description of
the five coursebooks under investigation. The data provided is grounded on a
careful reading of the coursebooks in question and their corresponding Teachers’
Manuals. Thus, there is no concern, at this stage, to develop a critical analysis of
the elements of the corpus.
85
3.2.1.1. The coursebook “ALFA” (1999, Oxford University Press)
The exemplar investigated has been selected from the fourth edition of the series
and is aimed at intermediate learners of English. According to its authors, this
publication presents a “clearly focused skills development” (Teachers’ Manual,
1999:9). Furthermore, the authors argue that the “well-planned” lessons are
genuinely effective in providing learners with tasks aimed at developing their
listening, speaking, reading and writing skills. The student’s book is divided into
eight units called “files”, which are “topic-based” (Teachers’ manual, 1999:9). At
the back of the student’s book, there is a thematically organized section entitled
“vocabulary builder” which focuses on the core topic-based vocabulary of the
course. In addition, the section entitled “build your vocabulary” consists of a
regular feature in the student’s book aimed at recycling the target lexical items
presented in the vocabulary builder. The tasks proposed are presented in a variety
of ways, including quizzes, memory tests and filling-in-the-gaps activities, among
others.
3.2.1.2. The coursebook “BETA” (1998. Cambridge University Press).
Coursebook “Beta” was published by the Cambridge University Press (henceforth
CUP) and has been widely used in different parts of the globe. The exemplar
investigated belongs to the second edition of the series first published in 1998. It is
also aimed at second/foreign language young adult/adult learners of English at the
intermediate level of EFL proficiency. The authors have argued strongly that the
edition to be investigated in the present research incorporates certain changes
86
suggested by teachers and learners from around the world (Teacher’s Manual,
1998: IV). According to this same teacher’s manual (op.cit., p. IV), the ultimate aim
of the course is to equip learners with the ability to communicate in a variety of
settings by considering the immediate situation, objectives and roles of the
participants involved. The authors claim that this primary goal is achieved through
their balanced focus on the development of the four skills as well as on
pronunciation and vocabulary building. Furthermore, the authors aver that the
design of the course is an attempt to reflect the relevance of the English language
as a means of international communication without being restricted to any
particular region, country or culture. The Student’s Book contains sixteen units.
The tasks proposed are grouped into two “cycles” i.e., topical and/or functional
sections.
3.2.1.3. The coursebook “GAMA” (1991. Longman.)
The series of coursebooks entitled “Gama” was first published in 1991 by the
Longman Group. The exemplar investigated is the ninth edition of the series and is
also aimed at intermediate adult learners of English. Among its key features, the
authors claim that certain topics and situations have been carefully chosen so as
to maximize the learners’ level of interest. The same authors also aver that the
publication aims at balancing fluency and accuracy by focusing on an equal
development of the four skills. It is also argued that learners are encouraged to
mobilize their background knowledge and it is claimed that by interacting with each
other the learners will be motivated to express their opinions on a variety of
“thought-provoking” topics.
87
3.2.1.4. The coursebook “Delta” (2003. Oxford University Press.)
Coursebook “Delta” was first published in 2003 by the Oxford University Press
(henceforth OUP). The exemplar investigated belongs to the second edition of the
series and is aimed at upper-intermediate young adult and/or adult learners of
English. According to its authors, this publication aims to promote a more natural
use of the target language. It is also claimed that this publication has resulted from
a research study in which communicative activities were piloted with upper-
intermediate and higher level learners in order to identify learners’ “coping
strategies”. The student’s book contains twelve units. Each unit has been divided
into five sections, namely “grammar & vocabulary”, “speaking”, “listening”,
“extended speaking” and “reading & writing” (Students’ Book, p. 2-5). The focus for
the present research will be the latter sections from the units selected.
3.2.1.5. The Coursebook “Épsilon” (1996. Heinemann)
Coursebook “Épsilon” was first published in 1996 by Heinemann Publishers. The
student’s book contains forty lessons, eight fluency lessons and eight progress
check lessons. American English is the model for grammar, vocabulary, spelling
and pronunciation, but other varieties of English are included for listening and
reading practice. The course design is claimed to be based on a broad and
integrated multi-syllabus approach. It is believed to be broad in the sense that it
arguably covers grammar and language functions, vocabulary, reading, listening,
speaking, writing, and sounds explicitly, and topics, learner training, and socio-
88
cultural competence implicitly. The lessons always include activities focusing on
grammar, language functions, and vocabulary.
3.2.2. The FCE reading papers
Each FCE (see Appendix 2) reading paper takes 75 minutes and consists of four
parts. Each part contains a text followed by its corresponding comprehension
tasks. There might be two or more shorter texts in one of the parts. However, the
texts are not usually lengthy. The number of words ranges from 350 to 700. The
total number of reading comprehension questions per paper is 35. According to
the FCE handbook (2001:9, UCLES, Cambridge) the reading paper attempts to
cater for a wide range of genres such as advertisements, brochures, manuals,
guides, newspaper/magazine articles and messages among other genres. The
types of questions proposed to candidates are usually multiple-choice, gapped
texts and multiple matching.
3.3. Collecting and analyzing data
3.3.1. Introduction
This section of the present research study is specifically concerned with the
analysis of the corpus. However, prior to proceeding to the analysis itself, it seems
important to discuss how the present investigation will be conducted. The
investigation consists of a three-staged analysis which comprises three distinct but
closely correlated stages. The first stage consists of an attempt to delineate the
discourse community at which the FCE Reading Paper is aimed. Therefore, in
89
order to either confirm or reject the initial hypothesis whereby FCE candidates,
exam designers, in addition to the institutions that prepare for the examination in
question, may be characterized as a discourse community, Swales’ (1990) six
defining features for the term were used as the theoretical foundation. At the
second stage however, the major concern is to provide a quantitative-interpretative
description of exemplars of the genre-texts identified in the reading sections of
coursebooks A, B, Γ, Δ and Ε (see Appendix 1). The same procedure will be
adopted with the genre-texts encountered in the four sets of FCE reading papers
(see Appendix 2). Salient features such as the communicative function, writer,
source and lexical, as well as syntactic choices which might help determine the
macro-patterns (Hoey, 1983; 2001), and which somehow contribute to the global
organization of the discourse, are also tackled. At this stage of the analysis,
Swales’ (1990, op. cit) investigations and “Working Definition of Genre” will serve
as the theoretical foundation in order to characterize the texts as exemplars of a
particular genre. The confrontation of findings from both sources may prove to be
revealing in the sense that this might determine a tendency towards particular
genres which, by the same token, might nurture the format and objectives of the
reading comprehension questions proposed to candidates. Furthermore, it is
possible to infer that the identification of the characteristic features of the genres
may provide EFL teachers in general with insights on how to exploit genres in their
reading classroom. The identification of the genres may function as a powerful tool
in order to prepare more effective and purposeful reading lessons therefore
enhancing learners’ self-confidence when approaching a wider range of genre-
texts. Finally, the third stage of the present investigation contemplates the reading
comprehension questions which accompany the twenty texts randomly chosen
90
from the five coursebooks. The same procedure will be adopted with the four FCE
reading papers. The ultimate aim at this stage consists of an attempt to determine
how genre-oriented the questions proposed to learners of English in general and
FCE candidates are. In the next sub-section each phase of the investigation will be
described in detail.
3.3.2. The Discourse Community behind the FCE examination
As discussed in sub-section 3.3.1 above, there will be an attempt at this first stage
of the analysis to delineate the discourse community which appears to nurture the
FCE examination. In order to achieve this aim, it has been decided to investigate
FCE candidates, exam designers, teachers and the institutions which prepare for
the examination in question in the light of the six defining features of a discourse
community postulated by Swales’ (1990):
1 – A discourse community has a broadly agreed set of common public goals.
The first defining feature of a discourse community, according to Swales, is that its
expert members should present a set of common goals. As far as the FCE
examination is concerned, this point appears to be closely related to the following
question: What is the common goal shared by The UCLES, exam designers,
institutions that prepare for the examination, teachers and FCE candidates?
Despite belonging to distinct domains and levels of expertise, it seems possible to
infer that the members previously mentioned appear to be concerned with the
materialization of a specific communicative event i.e., undergoing the FCE
91
examination. Thus, activities such as preparing and piloting exam papers prior to
their administration (i.e.: researchers and exam designers), training (i.e.: teachers
and institutions that prepare for the tests), applying (i.e.: examiners and
institutions) and undergoing the tests (i.e.: learners/candidates) may be
characterized as a set of goals all of which seem to be directly related to an
ultimate aim, namely passing the FCE.
Moreover, it seems important to note that the set of common goals appear to be
reinforced by the issues discussed in sub-sections 1.1. and 2.1.2. As highlighted,
the mastery of the English language has gained notorious relevance within the
global scenario. Thus, internationally recognized certification is often believed to
guarantee access to a wide range of benefits, mainly in academic and professional
settings. Moreover, certificate-holders are often viewed as representatives of a tiny
elite. They tend to be regarded as successful and competent individuals since
passing an international examination tends to be viewed as a demanding task.
2- A discourse community has mechanisms of intercommunication among its members.
As a result of the increase in the level of interest for the FCE examination, there
has been huge investment in the preparation phase for the exam in question. The
publishing industry has increased its profits through specialized publications aimed
at candidates, teachers and the institutions that prepare for the tests. Moreover,
with the advent of the internet, online training has also become common practice
among teachers and candidates who join discussion groups in which they discuss
aspects of the examination and how positive performance may be achieved. More
92
recently, specialized websites provide access to samples of past papers in order
to familiarize candidates’ with the format and types of questions. Similarly, private
language schools have also invested in this area by offering exam practices
courses which are often advertised as a sine-qua-non requisite for obtaining
positive results.
Another important factor to be considered in the candidates’ pursuit for success is
his/her economic power. Lopes (1999) addresses a candidate’s economic power
as a decisive element in order to obtain positive results in examinations. This
assumption is based on the fact that the preparation stage for international
examinations, for instance, often requires from candidates the acquisition of
expensive imported instructional materials such as exam practice books.
Moreover, the preparatory classes are usually held in expensive private language
schools which usually invite expert teachers to teach the lessons. Moreover,
classrooms are usually equipped with expensive pedagogical resources such as
computers, stereo systems etc. Similarly, candidates are expected to both devote
considerable “free time” to their studies and to be able to afford the fee to paid at
the enrollment phase. All these factors seem to suggest that less privileged
classes are usually at disadvantage.
3- A discourse community uses its participatory mechanisms primarily to provide
information and feedback.
Swales’ third defining feature acknowledges the relevance of the same
mechanisms of intercommunication among members as vehicles which may
regulate membership as well as guarantee that the members’ common goals are
93
being pursued. Moreover, participatory mechanisms may signal new routes to be
followed.
4- A discourse community utilizes and hence possesses one or more genres in the
communicative furtherance of its aims.
The fourth characteristic defining feature postulated by Swales may be illustrated
through the use of handouts, catalogues and leaflets which contain all the
information related to enrollment procedures (e.g.: fees to be paid by candidates,
places, dates and results). In addition, computer softwares and exam practice
books aimed at preparing for the examination are widely utilized as an aid to
preparation. This variety of informational and instructional materials seems to
require from candidates a familiarity with a wide range of genres. Even the FCE
test present particular characteristic features regarding format and discoursal
organization.
5- In addition to owning genres, a discourse community has acquired some specific
lexis.
As far as the FCE examination is concerned, the fifth defining feature advocated
by Swales may be verified through the use of specific “jargon” i.e., specific lexis
and acronyms. These elements might be claimed to be used for two major
reasons: a) to facilitate intercommunication among members and b) in order to
constrain the interference of non-members. Tables 3.1 and 3.2. (Appendices 3 and
4) might be regarded as highly illustrative of the need to be familiarized with a wide
range of specific jargon in order to become a member of the discourse community
which seems to nurture the FCE examination.
94
6 – A discourse community has a threshold level of members with a suitable degree of
relevant content and discoursal expertise.
As observed in Tables 3.1 and 3.2, as far as the level of expertise is concerned, it
seems possible to note that there seems to be a hierarchy among the members
which compose the FCE discourse community. The UCLES, for instance, is
responsible for the professional and specialist aspects of the examination itself. It
not only selects but also qualifies the EFL team of professionals who will make
sure that the examination reflects the level of excellence established by the
institution. Similarly, it is responsible for the administration and processing of
examinations. The researchers and exam designers also possess very specific
roles within this system. They not only design the tests but also check their
validation in order to ensure that the standards are being met and that the exams
reflect the changing needs of candidates. Moreover, these professionals develop
and implement training and monitoring procedures so that the examinations are
developed the same way regardless of the part of the world where it is being held.
Examiners consist of trained professionals whose major aim is to administer the
tests. Thus, they ought to be familiar with all the testing procedures. It may be
inferred therefore that the teachers who teach the exam practice lessons are
expected to be familiar with all the requirements established by the UCLES.
Furthermore, they should be qualified professionals who will be able to meet the
learners/candidates demands as far as preparation is concerned.
In the following sub-section a quantitative-interpretative analysis of the genre-texts
identified in both sources of the corpus of investigation will be conducted.
95
3.3.3. A quantitative-interpretative analysis of the genre-texts identified
3.3.3.1. In the coursebooks
As far as the variety and choice of genre-texts are concerned, the FCE question
designers (Teacher’s book, 2001) highlight the following premises:
1) FCE candidates are expected to be able to read semi-authentic texts of
various kinds (informative and general interest .... (p.02)
2) The texts are intended to cover a range of recently published material
and to appear authentic in form, presentation and content. (p.05)
3) Pictures and other diagrams are used where appropriate to illustrate
the text (...). Some of the vocabulary may be simplified in the texts to keep
it within the FCE level but such changes are kept to minimum. (p.05)
As highlighted in the introduction to the present chapter, the reading sections of
the coursebooks A, B, Γ, Δ and Ε will be focused upon in this subsection. The
ultimate aim at this stage is to determine the genre-texts which are predominant in
these sources. For the sake of brevity, there will be an attempt to exemplify how
the identification of each genre-text (see Appendix 5 for complete list of genre-
texts identified in the coursebooks) was conducted in the light of Swales’ (1990)
“Working Definition of Genre” by focusing on exemplars of the two predominant
genres, namely articles and stories. The decision to adopt Swales’ analytical
framework may be justified by its relevance to the field of genre analysis. As Lopes
(1999:83) claims, it provides sufficiently narrow criteria for genre analysis which allows
a more systematized analysis of the texts. The identification of one exemplar of each
predominant genre will be described in detail. In this way, an attempt will be made
96
towards portraying the procedures adopted in order to identify the exemplars of
the genres which constituted the 101 reading sections investigated (see table 3.3.
appendix 5, for the detailed list of exemplars of genres identified and their
corresponding number of occurrences). Furthermore, this secondary analysis aims
at providing a frame for the ultimate objective of the present research study, i.e.,
investigating the reading comprehension questions from the coursebooks and
FCE reading papers.
As observed, Table 3.3. (see Appendix 5) illustrates that, if, on one hand, a wide
range of genre-texts has been identified in the reading sections of coursebooks A,
B, Γ, Δ and Ε, there was no balanced distribution of the genre-texts among the
sources investigated. This question will be addressed in depth at a further stage in
this chapter. Moreover, as seen in Graph 1, below, magazine and/or newspaper
articles and stories constituted the two salient genre-texts in the corpus examined.
For this reason, the process undertaken throughout the identification of each
genre-exemplar highlighted in Table 3.3 (see Appendix 5) will be illustrated by
focusing on the two predominant genre-texts as seen in Graph 1.
articles
biographical sketches
brochures
conversations
interviews
letters
stories
questionnaires/quizzes
personal accounts
reviews
miscellaneous
articles
biographical sketches
brochures
conversations
interviews
letters
stories
questionnaires/quizzes
personal accounts
reviews
miscellaneous
Graph 1: Occurrences of genre-texts in the coursebooks
97
As highlighted in the preceding paragraphs, the identification of the genre-texts
was conducted in the light of Swales’ (1990) defining features for the term itself.
According to this author, a genre comprises a class of communicative events, the
members of which share some set of communicative purposes. These purposes are
recognized by the expert members of the parent discourse community and thereby
constitute the rationale for the genre.
As discussed in Chapter 2, language is usually governed by a given
communicative intent, i.e., the participants of a given communicative exchange
address one another through the linguistic medium. Interactants use language in
an agreed way (Tribble, 1996) in order to achieve a particular aim. As far as written
texts are concerned, writers shape their texts accordingly so as to convey their
communicative intent through printed symbols. As Tribble (1996) argues, each
communicative event requires a written text in order to be satisfactorily enacted. Thus,
the ultimate aim when analyzing the genre-texts provided in the coursebooks A, B,
Γ, Δ and Ε was to determine in the selected target texts the features which
unveiled their corresponding communicative purposes. In order to perform this
task, Swales’ definition of genre and Hoey’s (1983) organizational patterns for
texts analysis (see chapter 2) were borne in mind. Hoey’s (op. cit.) investigations
in this field provided with the rationale for the identification of the surface
characteristics/schematic structure of the target texts.
98
3.3.3.1.1. Sample Text ONE
Firstly, the focus will be on the magazine/newspaper articles. One exemplar of this
genre-text has been randomly chosen from coursebook A. The text is entitled Do
we really need to sleep? (see Appendix 6) and will be referred to hereafter as Text
ONE. The layout features constituted the starting point for the analysis as
displayed in Table 3.4 below:
(Table 3.4.: the layout features identified in Text ONE)
Text ONE
Layout features
The title is highlighted in bigger and different fonts from the main body of the text.
The particle “do we” is highlighted with a different color which distinguishes it from
the rest of the text.
There is a brief introductory paragraph before the main body of the text.
The text is displayed in four columns which resembles the layout through which
magazine and/or newspaper articles are presented.
Pictures have been used in order to illustrate the main topic of the text and arouse
readers’ level of interest.
The layout features outlined in Table 3.4, above, suggest that Text ONE might
have been taken either from a magazine or a newspaper. Since the source is not
provided, it is difficult to determine whether the text is authentic or semi-authentic,
i.e., if it has been modified in order to suit the coursebook writer’s main
communicative purpose. However, it may also be inferred that the numbering of
the lines suggest that the format might have been adapted in order to guide the
target audience when performing the reading comprehension questions related to
this particular reading section.
99
All in all, it may be argued that Text ONE appears to fulfill the FCE designers’
requirements (see p. above 95) as far as the choice of texts is concerned: the text
analyzed appears to be authentic in form even though the lines have been
numbered (see FCE premise 1, page 95). However, whether the article has been
recently published is difficult to determine in the sense that neither the source nor
the date of publication are provided (see FCE premise 2, page 95). A further key-
aspect to be considered, as far as the layout is concerned, is that pictures have
been added in order to illustrate the text (see FCE premise 3, page 95).
At this stage, as a means to identify the organizational pattern utilized by the
author in order to reach his/her communicative intent(s), Text ONE will be
investigated in the light of one of Hoey’s (2001) genre-general patterns. The
surface characteristics of the text in question suggest that it might be regarded as
a representative of the pattern “gap-in-knowledge-attempt-at-filling”. The
attempt at filling the gap in knowledge, i.e., whether sleeping is essential in human
beings’ lives, is introduced through the title of the text, which seems to aim at
arousing reader’s curiosity towards finding out an answer to the question
proposed. The situation, by the same token, is provided through the short text
which precedes the article itself: Tonight between eleven o’clock and one o’clock
millions of people will start yawning. Very soon, they will get undressed, lie down, and
close their eyes. A few minutes later, they will be asleep and through the introductory
paragraph (see lines 5 to21) in the main body of the text.
The attempt at filling the gap in knowledge is presented in line 22 through the use
of the connector however which introduces three key statements: (1) However, Ray
Meddis, a scientist at the Sleep Research Unit at Loughborough University, has a
100
fascinating new theory. (2) He suggests that we don’t really need to sleep at all. (3) We
sleep only because our brain is “programmed” to make us do so. Furthermore, the
author supports the new theory by highlighting that:
a) the primitive man was “programmed” to sleep to protect himself from the darkness
with its many dangers. (lines 33-37)
b) Animals appear to have been similarly programmed and the number of hours that they
sleep does not depend on physical activity but on how much time they need to eat.
(lines 37-43)
c) We are “programmed” to feel tired or sleepy at midnight... (lines 56-57)
The exemplification above attempts at filling the gap in knowledge and as a result
reinforces Dr. Ray Meddis’ theory whereby scientists could locate and “turn off” the
sleep mechanism in our brain so that we could live completely normal and healthy lives
without sleeping. (lines 68-74)
It may thus be argued that the schematic structure (Swales, 1990: 84-87) of the
text in question resembles a pseudo-scientific article which aims at filling a gap in
the target audience’s knowledge. As far as the concluding paragraph is
concerned, in an attempt to get readers involved, questions are raised, though not
answered. It may thus be inferred that a new genre-general pattern is introduced:
the problem-response-solution pattern (Hoey, 2001:123-139). These findings
seem to corroborate the view whereby there are usually overlapping patterns
within a given larger clause-relational analysis (Winter, 1977, apud Hoey,
1986:188) of the same piece of writing: ...clause relations are defined in terms of
101
writer and reader, they may be seen as abstractions from the questions a writer seeks to
answer, and a reader sees being answered, in a discourse (Hoey, 1986:189).
Alternatively, the identification of the pattern outlined above might have been
conducted by projecting the discourse in question into a question-and-answer
dialog (Hoey, 1986:191) as represented in Table 3.5 below:
(Table 3.5: the identification of macro-patterns via question-and-answer dialog)
Macro-pattern identified
+
questions
Evidence in text ONE
Gap in knowledge
(What do readers need to know about sleep ?)
Do we really need to sleep ? (title)
Situation
(What do people know about sleep until today ?)
Sleep is a powerful influence on all
our lives... (lines 5 and 6)
The traditional theory about sleep is
that our brain needs to rest for
several hours ... the day. (lines 8 to
14)
... sooner or later we have to sleep.
(lines 18 and 19)
Attempt at filling
(What is really “true” about sleep ?)
However, Ray Meddis... has a
fascinating new theory.(lines 22 to 26)
Evaluation and/or result
(What was the result/evaluation of the research?)
no evaluation is provided
another pattern takes over
Another central aspect to be considered as far as text analysis is concerned, is to
what extent both grammatical and lexical choices enable readers to identify the
relations between the clauses and, at a further stage, the macro-pattern of which a
particular text may be a representative. The analytical findings for Text ONE have
been listed in Table 3.6 below:
102
(Table 3.6: sample of Lexicogrammatical choices in text ONE.)
Samples of Lexicogrammatical features in text ONE
Generic human and non-
human participants
Sleep, traditional theory, hallucinations, a scientist, a new
theory, primitive man, darkness, dangers, animals.
Intensive Sleep is a powerful influence
The traditional theory about sleep is that...
Relational processes
possessive A scientist (...) has a fascinating theory.
Cats (...) have a lot of spare time.
Internal conjunction to state
argument
For instance, for example, however, on the other hand,
according to, even if, because but.
Use of simple present tense
Is, needs, go, suggests, has, believes, spend, suffer.
Causal conjunction
or
nominalisation
A few minutes later, they will be asleep.
(...) but sooner or later we have to sleep.
So is sleeping a waste of time?
A further aspect to be highlighted is that since this consists of an argumentative
text, in which the writer takes a position on a particular issue, and attempts to
persuade the reader to agree with his/her theory, verbs which describe mental
processes such as those in the statements a) He believes that the sleep instinct
originates from prehistoric times...; b) Dr Meddis believes that the unpleasant symptoms
we suffer (...) and c) But Dr Meddis believes that if scientists could locate and turn off the
sleep mechanism are used. Moreover the mentioning of the title Dr and the name of
the Sleep Research Unit at Loughborough University seem to provide
reliability/prestige for the argument in the text.
3.3.3.1.2. Sample Text TWO
The exemplar of the second salient genre-text identified in the coursebooks
investigated consists of a story randomly chosen from coursebook B. The text is
entitled Friends again – forever ! (see Appendix 7) and henceforth will be addressed
103
as Text TWO. The layout features have been investigated as displayed in Table
3.7 below:
(Table 3.7: the layout features identified in text TWO)
Text TWO
Layout features
The title is highlighted in bigger and different fonts from the main body of the text.
A question in smaller fonts is provided before the main body of the text in order to get
the target audience interested in reading the text.
The text is displayed in two columns.
Two pictures have been provided (one in colors and the other in black and white) in
order to illustrate the main topic of the text and arouse readers’ level of interest.
The layout features outlined in Table 3.7 appear to suggest that Text TWO might
have been taken from a magazine. As far as authenticity is concerned, it is difficult
to state whether the text is authentic or semi-authentic since neither the source nor
the date of publication are provided. Furthermore, since the source is not provided,
it is not possible to determine whether the language has been modified in order to
suit the learners’ level of proficiency. Unlike genre-text ONE, the sentences in
TWO have not been numbered. In general terms, it is possible to infer that text
TWO appears to fulfill the FCE designers’ requirements (see page 95 above), as
far as their three premises are concerned.
As to the schematic structure of Text TWO, it is possible to argue that the text in
question appears to be a representative of the problem-response-solution
pattern advocated by Hoey (2001, op.cit.). The identification of the rhetorical
moves has been conducted as summarized in the question-and-answer dialog
(Hoey, 1986:191) transcribed in Table 3.8:
104
(Table 3.8.: the identification of macro-patterns via question-and-answer dialog)
Macro-pattern identified
+
questions
Evidence in text TWO
Situation
(What was the situation like between the two
childhood friends ?)
Mary Allen was my best friend (...), we saw
each other on special occasions – like my
wedding and Mary’s.
Problem
(What problem arose after the two friends
had children and moved to their new homes?)
Soon we were busy with children and
moving to new homes (...). There was an
empty place in my heart that only a friend
like Mary could fill.
Response
(What did the writer decide to do when she
saw the photo on the newspaper ?)
One day I was reading the newspaper
when I noticed a photo of a young
woman (...). We laughed and cried and
caught up on each other’s lives.
Solution/evaluation
(How successful was the result ?)
Now the empty place in my heart is filled
(...). We won’t lose each other again !
As observed in Table 3.8, the questions asked appear to focus reader attention on
the generic problem-response-solution pattern. The situation (see table 3.8
above) answers a number of key questions such as what, who, where and when.
The situation sets the scene of the narrative by providing details of what the story
is about (i.e.: two female childhood friends who had taken separate ways); who the
individuals/elements involved in the plot are (i.e.: the two female childhood
friends); where the narrative takes place. In this story the setting may be inferred
from the co-text. In its first cycle, for instance, the story might have taken place in
the neighborhood where the two friends lived. Although information regarding
when the events happened is not overtly mentioned in the text, readers may infer
from the co-text that the story happened a long time ago, at an indefinite time in
the past. The problem is introduced in the selected text by means of syntactic and
lexical choices. Valuable examples of both elements are the clauses Soon we were
busy with children... and we wrote less often, which appear to signal a change in the
105
text and somehow prepare the reader for something negative that is likely to
happen later on: ... a card that I sent came back stamped “address unknown” and I had
no idea how to find Mary. Similarly, the expressions one day and over the years also
constitute signals of change in the narrative. Lexical choices such as address
unknown, empty place and sorrow as well as verb choices such as needed and died
also reveal the problem. The response conveys what finally happened according
to the problem previously described in the narrative. Among the syntactic and
lexical choices which signal the response, it is worth highlighting wrote and anyway,
respectively. As to the evaluation, evaluative terms have been used as a means to
express how successful the response to the problem was. It seems possible to
argue that the evaluation is positive as the connection between the expressions
empty place and my heart is filled appear to reveal. The past simple is the major verb
tense observable throughout the analysis of Text TWO. Samples of the significant
lexico-grammatical features in Text TWO are displayed in Table 3.9 below:
(Table 3.9: samples of lexico-grammatical choices in text TWO.)
Samples Lexicogrammatical features in text TWO
Specific participants
Mary, best friend, I, we, she
Material processes
Wrote, did, moved, sent, was reading, called, laughed and
cried
time
When I was 13...; on special occasions; over the
years
Circumstances
place
To new homes
Temporal sequence
When, and, one day, then, when, now
In view of the considerations presented in the preceding paragraph, it seems
possible to argue that the questions asked by readers enable them to recognize
the connection between the sentences: The questions spell out the relationship
between the sentences (Hoey, 1986:191). According to Hoey (op. cit.), there is
certainly a wide range of possible formulations for questions to be asked by
readers. Thus, the questions presented in table 3.5 and Table 3.8 aim at
106
representing a sample within a realm of alternatives. A further aspect to be
highlighted, as far as the formulation of questions is concerned, is that such
questions usually present very specific vocabulary (Winter, 1977, apud Hoey
1986:192). This specific vocabulary aims at signaling the pattern(s) which emerge
from the clause-relational analysis. In the questions presented in Table 3.8, for
instance, it is possible to highlight the following lexical items: situation, problem,
decide to and result. These signals are not restricted to the questions though. They
may be identified within the main body of the discourse itself.
A further aspect which is worth highlighting is that the pattern unveiled in Table 3.8
(i.e.: Situation > Problem > Response > Positive Evaluation) fails to reflect the
sequence in which the sentences are placed, but, rather, reflects the pattern of
relations within the discourse. Thus, as Hoey (1986:195) advocates, some of the
elements in patterns of organization appear in various sequences, and one element may be
embedded within another. From this perspective, it is possible to argue that the
clause but I wrote to her anyway might be interpreted as a response to the problem
introduced by the clause there must be thousands of Wagmans, both inserted in the
response macro-move. Similarly, the clause soon we were busy with children and
moving to new homes, and we wrote less often might be interpreted as a situation
within the problem macro-move. This suggests that a particular text may be
scrutinized from two distinct and, at the same time, closely correlated
perspectives, namely, by considering its macro-structure and, by considering its
clause-relational properties. The latter functions as a facilitator in identifying the
macro-moves.
107
3.3.4. Predominant genre-texts
According to the FCE handbook (2001:10), the ultimate goal of the FCE Reading
Paper may be summarized along the following lines:
The range of texts and task types is intended to encourage a familiarity
with many different types of reading material and also the use of different
approaches to reading.
The UCLES FCE handbook (2001:10) draws teachers’ and examiners’ attention to
the variety regarding the choices of genre-texts used in the FCE Reading Papers.
According to these premises, the texts come from a wide range of sources and
include a number of genres such as advertisements, guides, messages, reports,
brochures, newspaper and magazine articles. In their view it is thus desirable that
potential FCE candidates have contact with a wide range of texts which display the
prototypical features of the genres to which they belong. Thus, they should be
familiar with a number of varied reading materials so as to respond to the tasks
proposed accordingly. From this perspective, it may be inferred that reading-skills
books and coursebooks are expected to contain representatives of a diversity of
genre-texts so as to expand learner’s genre-schemata and, as a consequence,
build-up their self-confidence in tackling a range of texts.
Therefore, candidates should be encouraged to read purposefully (UCLES FCE
handbook, 2001:10). Thus, our ultimate aim at this stage is to determine which
genre-texts which are predominant in the coursebooks investigated. This
procedure may be justified by the need to verify whether the genre-texts identified
in our corpus reflect the demands of the FCE Reading Paper. The graphs below
display the percentage of occurrences of each genre-text identified in the reading
108
sections of the corpus examined (see appendix 5 for the complete list of genre-
texts identified).
Graph 2: predominant genre-texts in coursebook “alfa”.
Coursebook "alfa"
40%
25%
10%
25%
articles stories
personnal accounts miscellaneous
Graph 4: predominant genre-texts in coursebook “gama”.
Coursebook "gama"
10%
13%
10%
10%
17%
10%
13%
17%
articles biographical sketches
miscellaneous conversations
letters stories
personal accounts miscellaneous
Coursebook "beta"
68%
13%
19%
articles stories miscellaneous
Graph 3: predominant genre-texts in coursebook “beta”.
109
3.3.5. Preliminary conclusions
As the graphs above demonstrate, among the five coursebooks investigated,
coursebook “Gama” (see Graph 4, page 108) seems to be the only source which
contained a balanced distribution of genre-texts. The most noticeable
discrepancies have been identified in the other four sources (i.e.: coursebooks
Graph 5: predominant genre-texts in coursebook “delta”.
Coursebook "delta"
92%
8%
0%
articles stories miscellaneous
Graph 6: predominant genre-texts in coursebook “épsilon”.
Coursebook "épsilon"
49%
8%
8%
8%
12%
15%
articles letters
stories questionnaires/quizzes
personal accounts miscellaneous
110
“Alfa”, “Beta”, “Delta” and “Épsilon”), which contained a high percentage of either
newspaper and/or magazine articles (i.e.: 40%, 68%, 92% and 49%, respectively).
In a nutshell, it may be argued that determining the exemplars of the predominant
genre-text was a particularly painstaking task, as most texts appear to have been
modified in their layout to resemble articles. Therefore, alongside with the internal
organization/rhetorical moves of the texts, it was necessary to focus on the layout
features of the target texts. This decision may be justified by the fact that the
internal organization, which determined both lexical and syntactic choices in the
target texts, could have also been interpreted as defining features of a variety of
other genres, rather than newspaper and/or magazine articles. The figure below
aims at exemplifying the variety of genres which have been displayed in the format
of newspaper/magazine articles in the corpus investigated:
As it may be observed in Figure 1, genres such as biographical sketches (see
Appendix 8), travel brochures (see Appendix 9) and interviews (see Appendix 10)
have been displayed in the format of newspaper/magazine articles. This finding
seems to suggest that articles could be interpreted as an “umbrella genre” or as a
“primary genre” which incorporates other sub-genre texts. The supremacy of either
Magazine/newspaper
articles
Interviews
Biographical
sketches
Figure 1: Genre-texts presented in the format of articles
Stories
Brochures
Personal
accounts
111
newspaper and/or magazine articles in the sources investigated has led to the
following questions:
a) Does the predominance of newspaper/magazine articles reveal a tendency in
the global editorial market?
b) Does this predominance signal that newspaper/magazine articles constitute the
most popular genre among readers in general?
c) Is it possible to argue that the predominance of newspaper/magazine articles in
the coursebooks investigated reflect the predominance of the same genres in
the FCE Reading Paper ?
d) Even though the coursebooks investigated do not constitute exemplars of
exam practice books, would it be possible to claim that, among other
objectives, such coursebooks are aimed at building up learners’ genre-
schemata for the FCE Reading Paper ?
An attempt to determine the salient genre-texts in the coursebooks investigated
would seem to constitute a subsidiary aim in this research. However, the questions
outlined above serve as “good food for thought” in the sense that they are likely to
reveal the parameters which determine the choice of genre-texts to be presented
to learners/FCE candidates. On the other hand, no doubt further investigations
focusing on the choice of genre-texts will be necessary in order to determine the
rationale behind such choice. Therefore, for the purpose of the present
investigation, a quantitative-interpretative analysis of the genre-texts utilized in the
FCE Reading Paper will be made, and, by confronting the data obtained through
the analysis of the coursebooks, conclusions will be drawn on what the findings
appear to suggest.
112
3.3.6. Predominant genre-texts in the FCE reading paper.
The tables presented below aim at summarizing the findings regarding the choice
of genre-texts. These findings emerged from the detailed investigation of the four
FCE Reading Papers which constitute the corpus of analysis (see Appendix 2).
(Table 3.10: salient genre-texts identified in the FCE Reading paper: Form A – June 2003)
Genre-text identified Title Source
Part 1
Magazine article Absolute beginners Not provided
Part 2
Magazine article Not provided Not provided
Part 3
Magazine article
Cooking for the
camera
Not provided
Part 4
Magazine article Speak easy Not provided
(Table 3.11: salient genre-texts identified in the FCE Reading paper: Form A – December 2003)
Genre-text identified Title Source
Part 1
Magazine article Memory test Not provided
Part 2
Magazine article
Acting minus the
drama
Not provided
Part 3
Magazine article The runningman Not provided
Part 4
Magazine article A new life Not provided
(Table 3.12: salient genre-texts identified in the FCE Reading paper: Form A – June 2004)
Genre-text identified Title Source
Part 1
Magazine article Summer job abroad Not provided
Part 2
Magazine article Bringing up Rupert Not provided
Part 3
Magazine article an artist at work Not provided
Part 4
Magazine article Women in pop music Not provided
113
(Table 3.13: salient genre-texts identified in the FCE Reading paper: Form A – December 2004)
Genre-text identified Title Source
Part 1
Magazine article Funny faces Not provided
Part 2
Magazine article
Growing up with the
violin
Not provided
Part 3
Magazine article Taking care of bears Not provided
Part 4
Magazine article My attitude to work Not provided
The procedures adopted for the identification of the salient genre-texts presented
in the FCE Reading papers were identical to those adopted for the identification of
the predominant genre-texts in the coursebooks (see Sections 3.3.3 and 3.3.4 for
detailed description of procedures). As may be observed in the tables presented
above, a preliminary analysis revealed that all texts in the FCE Reading Papers
investigated constitute examples of “articles” (see discussion on the modification
of generic layout features in Section 3.3.5, pp 110 and111). The data obtained
was confronted with the three premises established by FCE question designers
(see Sub-section 3.3.3.1., p. 95 for a comprehensive description of such
premises). As far as the three premises for choice of texts are concerned, it is
possible to argue that:
Premise 1: According to this premise, FCE candidates are expected to be able to read
semi-authentic texts of various kinds (Teacher’s book, 2001:2). It is worth questioning
at this stage what is meant by the term “semi-authentic”. It is difficult to determine
whether the expression is being used to refer to texts which have had their lexico-
grammatical features modified from the original text (i.e.: made deliberately easier)
in order to suit the FCE demands (i.e.: the candidates’ proficiency level), or which
have had their original forms modified (i.e.: a story changed into an article, for
instance). Furthermore, it is also difficult to determine what is meant by “various
114
kinds”. This expression might have been used to refer to either textual genres (i.e.:
brochures, interviews etc) or text types (i.e.: argumentative, descriptive). The fact
that in the FCE handbook, the terms “text type” and “textual genres” have been
used interchangeably is revealed by the extract transcribed below:
Text Types
From the following: advertisements, correspondence, fiction,
informational material (e.g.: brochures, guides, manuals, etc.),
messages, newspaper and magazine articles, reports. (FCE
HANDBOOK, 2001:9)
Premise two: It seems difficult to determine whether the texts proposed to
candidates come from recently published material since neither the source nor the
date of publication are provided. However, the texts appear authentic in form,
presentation and content (Teacher’s book, 2001:5).
Premise three: Certain sample texts present pictures and other diagrams as
illustrations. It may be inferred that these elements have been utilized in order to
activate candidates’ schemata and/or arouse the level of interest regarding the
issues to be discussed in the target text.
3.3.7. Preliminary conclusions
According to the premises established in the FCE Handbook, FCE candidates are
expected to be able to tackle a wide range of genres. In fact, a variety of genres
was identified. However, most genre-texts were presented to candidates in the
format of articles. This seems to suggest that, in common with the coursebooks
115
investigated, the umbrella or primary genre, denominated “articles”, appears to
have been used in both sources as an “umbrella genre” which embodies a wide
range of other genres. The layout features in which stories are presented, for
instance, resemble that of the articles. However, the investigation of the rhetorical
moves/internal organization and lexico-grammatical features seem to suggest that
the texts examined might be representatives of a range of distinct genres.
Furthermore, the genres to which the texts used in the FCE belong is clearly
stated in the rubric to the tasks (see Appendix 2). Thus the rubric activates the
readers´ schemata in terms of the kind of genre they are about to read. However,
the mentioning of the genre to be presented in the rubric is somewhat problematic
in the sense that it might mislead the reader towards activating inappropriate
schemata.
3.3.8. Analysis of the reading comprehension questions
3.3.8.1. Introduction
As stated in previous sections, the bulk of the present investigation lies in
identifying and discussing the degree to which the reading comprehension
questions proposed for learners of English at the intermediate level of proficiency
and FCE candidates are genre-oriented. Thus, there will be an attempt to
investigate whether the questions proposed reflect the communicative purposes of
the genres to which the target texts belong. In other words, this research aims at
verifying the extent to which the questions which accompany the target texts are
aimed at measuring learners’ and FCE candidates’ awareness regarding genre
116
features (see Section 2.3 for a detailed discussion on this topic). The results might
serve to indicate whether coursebook writers and FCE Reading Paper designers
approach the question of genre as an integrating element when assessing the
development of effective reading skills.
A preliminary analysis of the corpus for analysis revealed that the reading
comprehension questions proposed for learners and FCE candidates seem to
serve to a wide range of purposes, which have been labeled as follows:
1) testing the knowledge of grammar;
2) testing vocabulary recognition;
3) testing the knowledge of grammatical and/or lexical cohesion;
4) testing the identification of specific information in the target text (i.e.,
scanning);
5) testing the identification of the overall message of the target text (i.e.,
skimming);
6) testing the awareness of discourse organizational features (i.e. coherence);
7) testing the knowledge of generic features (i.e.: aspects of field, tenor and
mode);
For analytical purposes, the variety of testing purposes previously outlined was
divided into two major arenas: a) non-genre oriented questions (i.e.: purposes 1, 2
and 4) and b) genre-oriented questions (i.e.: questions aimed at assessing the
knowledge of genre features) (purposes 3, 5, 6 and 7). These two categories will
be described in detail in the following sub-sections.
117
3.3.8.2. Analyzing data
The following sub-sections are aimed at exemplifying the occurrence of the testing
purposes outlined in the previous section. The choice of the sample questions
transcribed below was determined by the fact that they appear to constitute typical
evidence of each testing purpose. For a full view of the reading comprehension
questions from the corpus of analysis, see appendices 1 and 2.
3.3.8.2.1. The Non-genre oriented questions
The non-genre oriented questions seem to comprise the reading comprehension
questions which seem to be specifically related to the identification of information
which is overtly expressed in parts of the text (Purpose 4) or questions which are
concerned with the recognition of grammatical and/or lexical items (Purposes 1
and 2). It seems important to highlight that reading comprehension questions
should not be restricted to Purposes 1, 2 and 4. They are expected to require
readers to interact a wider range of knowledge including linguistic (i.e.:
grammatical and lexical knowledge), textual (i.e.: awareness of discourse
organizational features), as well as background knowledge in order to infer,
understand and/or synthesize ideas, facts, values and beliefs (Lopes, 1999:192).
As far as linguistic knowledge is concerned, it seems important to highlight that
even though grammatical items and lexical items are said to belong to distinct
systems of the language (i.e., the former would be related to the closed systems
whereas the latter would be treated as belonging to the open systems), they seem
118
to be integrating elements of the same phenomenon: combining form and function
in order to construct meanings (McCarthy, 1991; Halliday & Hasan, 1985, apud
Lopes, op. cit.).
a) Testing the knowledge of grammar:
3. Find the examples of
though
and
although
in the text. Complete the sentences in your own
words. Compare with a partner.
1. _________________. It rained all the time, though.
2. _________________. The journey back was terrible, though.
3. Travelling can be fun. _________________, though.
4. Dieter Graf’s book looks interesting. _________________, though.
(See Appendix 1, Coursebook Delta, Text 1)
As may be observed, the question transcribed above fails to develop either the
learners’ reading skills or their awareness of generic features. It may be inferred
that this question seems specifically concerned with the teaching/testing of
grammatical knowledge. The reading of the target text thus seems to have as its
ultimate aim the identification of the elements of grammar which are necessary in
order to fill in gaps. Moreover, even though it is suggested that learners read the
text prior to answering the question, it is possible to argue that the reading of the
text does not constitute a sine-qua-non procedure in order to perform the task. In
actual fact, learners might well be able to perform the task even without reading
the text provided that they had already been introduced to the use of though and
although.
119
b) Testing vocabulary recognition:
The question transcribed above seems to illustrate the testing/teaching of
vocabulary. Both the reading of the target text as well, as a knowledge of
discourse organizational features (i.e., aspects of cohesion and coherence), are
necessary in order to identify the unknown lexical items or expressions.
Furthermore, meanings may be inferred by resorting to the co-text.
C. In pairs, underline all the new words or expressions in the text, and try to guess their
meaning from the context. Now find a word or expression which means:
Paragraph 1
1. having or using something with another person (v.) ................................
Paragraph 2
2. person you share a flat with (n.) ................................
3. began living in a flat or house (v.) ................................
4. discovered (v.) ................................
5. always worried about (adj.) ................................
6. having everything in the right place (adj.) ................................
Paragraph 3
7. some, but not many (adj.) ................................
Paragraph 4
8. annoying (adj.) ................................
9. very tired (adj.) ................................
(
See A
pp
endix 1, Coursebook Alfa, Text 1
)
120
c) Testing the identification of specific information in the target text (i.e.,
scanning):
1.
A. Read the article. Then answer these questions.
1. How has Silver Cliff changed over the years ?
2. When, where, and by whom were the blue lights first seen ?
3. What do the blue lights look like ?
4. What are some of the explanations that people have proposed ?
(See Appendix 1, Coursebook Beta, Text 4)
2.
3. Answer these questions.
1. How long has Batman been saving Gotham City from evil ?
2. How long has Charlie Brown been living in a small American town ?
3. How long has Bugs Bunny been eating carrots in public ?
4. How long has kermit been singing and dancing on television ?
5. How many Batman TV series has the team made ?
6. How many Charlie Brown movies have there been ?
7. How many cartoon movies has Bugs Bunny appeared in ?
8. How many Muppet shows have people seen ?
(See Appendix 1, Coursebook Épsilon, Text 1)
3.
You are going to read a magazine article about five women pop musicians. For questions 23-35,
choose from the musicians (A-E). The musicians may be chosen more than once. When more
than one answer is required, these may be given in any order. There is an example at the
beginning (0).
...
Which musician(s)
Lost her job ?
Advises people not to give up easily ?
Says she was encouraged to be ambitious ?
Says her first job came as a result of some publicity ?
Says that people took a while to accept her ?
Says she wasn’t forced to learn an instrument ?
121
Dislikes certain aspects of her job ?
Was worried about what would happen when she joined a band ?
Admits that her early music lessons were disappointing ?
Mentions the difficulty of adjusting to a normal lifestyle ?
Says she enjoys making decisions ?
Says she isn’t very outgoing ?
Were offered a temporary position in a band ?
(See Appendix 2, FCE June/2004, Part 4)
Even though the identification of the pieces of information required from
learners/FCE candidates seem to derive from a multiplicity of inferential
processes, it may be argued that the answers to the questions proposed above
spring mainly from the recognition and decodification of printed symbols. In order
to perform Tasks 1 and 2 above, for example, learners/FCE candidates should be
attentive to the use of the question words such as how, when, where, what and
which. These words seem to signal the information which is required as in the
question When, where, and by whom were the blue lights first seen? whose answer is
overtly mentioned in the body of the target text through the expressions late one
night in 1880, a graveyard on a hill and a group of miners, respectively. Question 3,
however, requires the identification of synonymous words, expressions or ideas
such as the relation between the phrases lost her job and got sacked (Appendix 2,
FCE June/2004, Part 4, extract B); was encouraged to be ambitious and the sentence
My mum was a brilliant executive who had got to the top, and she made me believe that the
sky was the limit (Appendix 2, FCE June/2004, Part 4, extract A) and the phrases
her first job came as a result of some publicity and one day the local paper did a piece on
me and my sisters’ group (...) I was asked to join the group full-time (Appendix 2, FCE
June/2004, Part 4, extract C).
122
3.3.8.2.2. The genre-oriented questions
As discussed in Subsection 2.3., above, the rationale of a genre seems to govern
the rhetorical and structural organization of a given text. It influences register
variables related to aspects of field, tenor and mode. Furthermore, it seems to
constrain the content of a particular communicative event.
Thus, in very general terms, it may be argued that the genre-oriented questions
(Purposes 3, 5, 6 and 7, above) proposed to learners/candidates should
contemplate the aspects of genre such as the identification of the communicative
purpose(s) as well as the aspects of field, tenor and mode. Similarly, they are also
likely to contemplate salient layout features which are might be revealing of the
genre to which they refer. If the questions contemplate at least one of the aspects
previously mentioned, they may be considered as representatives of genre-
oriented questions.
a) Testing the knowledge of grammatical and/or lexical cohesion:
1.
Read better
a. Read two mixed-up stories:
The Wedding
and
The Interview
. Which four paragraphs
belong to each story ? Write the correct order 1-4.
The wedding 1........ 2........ 3........ 4........
The Interview 1........ 2........ 3........ 4........
(See Appendix 1, Coursebook Alfa, Text 3)
123
2.
Reading
Read the five parts of the following story. See if you notice anything unusual.
...
1. You probably noticed that the five parts of the story are not in the right order. Put them
in the order you think is correct.
(See Appendix 1, Coursebook Gama, Text 3)
3.
11. “This” (line 21) refers to
A The book department.
B a confusing situation.
C the assistants’ free choice of clothes.
D Mr Parker’s attitude to customers.
(See Appendix 2, FCE June/2003, Part 2)
4.
You are going to read a newspaper article about a man who is running round the world. Eight
paragraphs have been removed from the article. Choose from the paragraphs A-I the one which fits
each gap (16-22). There is one extra paragraph which you do not need to use. There is an
example at the beginning (0).
(See Appendix 2, FCE December/2003, Part 3)
The four questions transcribed above seem to reflect the fact that the
teaching/testing of both grammatical and/or lexical cohesion may be performed in
a variety of ways. As may be seen, in order to perform the tasks transcribed
above, learners and/or FCE candidates should be attentive to the clues spread
along the text’s surface. The four questions seem to draw learners/candidates
attention to elements of both grammatical and lexical cohesion. Furthermore,
aspects of coherence are of crucial importance in the sense that they determine
the organization of information at the sentence level and between paragraphs in a
coherent way, so as to construct meanings. Thus, in question 1, for example, it
124
may be inferred that lexical items such as ceremony, elegant suit and best man seem
to correspond to the extract entitled The Wedding whereas the lexical items
interview, job and manager seem to be related to the extract entitled The interview. In
addition, in order to put each of the paragraphs in the appropriate chronological
sequence, learners would need to focus on the use of connectors for explicitness
such as but, although and five minutes later, among others.
b) Testing the identification of the overall message of the target text (i.e.,
skimming):
1.
2. Match these headings with the paragraphs in the article.
- taking care of my diet - emotional support
- providing physical help - how I ended up in hospital
- life in a typical hospital
(See Appendix 1, Coursebook Delta, Text 3)
2.
You are going to read a magazine article about an orchestra. Choose the most suitable heading
from the list A-I for each part (1-7) of the article. There is one extra heading which you do
not need to use. There is an example at the beginning (0)
A Not as silly as it sounds
B Not the original intention
C Responding to a demand
D A great discovery for many
D Leading to new ambitions
F Modest origins
G Great dedication
H Nobody is excluded
I The orchestra you can join straight away
(See Appendix 2, FCE June/2003, Part 1)
125
Both questions transcribed above seem to illustrate the testing of the readers’
ability to infer the writer’s intended meanings. The questions seem to be aimed at
measuring the overall comprehension of the extracts to which the headings are
related.
c) Testing the awareness of discourse organizational features (i.e.:
coherence):
1. You’re going to read about a woman who participated in a scientific experiment. Put these
paragraph topics in a logical order. Compare with a partner.
a. the conditions she lived in for a month
b. the purpose of the experiment
c. the after-effects of the experiment
d. how scientists created the conditions for the experiment
e. her feelings during the experiment.
(See Appendix 1, Coursebook Delta, Text 2)
The question transcribed above seems to illustrate the identification of a logical
discourse structure, which is likely to unveil the communicative purpose of the
target text. Moreover, it may be argued that in order to perform this task,
learners/candidates are expected to resort to their genre-schemata in an attempt
to identify the logical sequencing which would be characteristic of this particular
socially recognized genre. This appears to suggest that the ability to identify genre
features requires a familiarity with a variety of genres since it is the communicative
purpose which determines the content, the grammatical and lexical patterns used
and ultimately, the overall organization.
126
d) Testing the knowledge of generic features (i.e.: aspects of field, tenor and
mode):
Study the three texts and answer these questions.
1. One is a diary entry. What are the other two ?
2. Where would you find a text like “A dead film about a dead actor ?”
...
4. Who sent the wedding announcement ?
6. Think about the diary entry. Which person on the plane wrote this ? What is the writer’s
father like ?
(See Appendix 1, Coursebook Gama, Text 1)
The questions transcribed above are related to three different genres: a movie
review, a wedding announcement and diary entry. Those questions aim at
illustrating the testing/checking learners’ and/or candidates’ awareness regarding
generic features. Question 1, for example, seems to be concerned with the
identification of the communicative purposes of the target texts. In the rubric, one
of the target texts is identified as a diary entry and learners are required to identify
the other two genre-texts. In order to perform this task, learners and/or candidates
are expected to be familiar with the kind of overall organization which
characterizes those particular genres. Learners and/or candidates must be aware
of the fact that the rationale of the genre not only determines their schematic
organization but also the grammatical and lexical choices so as to shape
meanings accordingly. Question 2, however, appears to focus on the aspects of
mode i.e., the type of publication where readers are likely to find the genre-text
entitled A dead film about a dead actor. Similarly, Questions 4 and 6 seem to focus
on the identification of interpersonal meanings (tenor) i.e. the individuals involved
in the interaction.
127
3.3.9. Main findings: contrasting data
3.3.9.1. Introduction
The analyses and subsequent identification of the reading comprehension
questions from both sources of the corpus have generated the urge to quantify the
proportion of genre-oriented questions in contrast to non-genre oriented questions.
It may be argued that the figures and percentages identified are likely to reveal the
extent to which the question of genre is contemplated in both sources of the
corpus. Furthermore, this procedure might unveil the rationale which appears to
nurture the choice and elaboration of reading comprehension questions.
3.3.9.2. The coursebooks
The investigation of the comprehension questions in the five coursebooks from the
corpus has led to the elaboration of Table 3.14 below:
(Table 3.14: an overview of the comprehension questions identified in the coursebooks)
Reading comprehension questions ¾
Coursebooks investigated À
Genre-oriented
Non-genre
oriented
ALFA 1 14
BETA 0 7
GAMA 7 18
DELTA 2 15
ÉPSILON 6 5
Total 16 59
As the figures presented in the table above suggest, there seems to be a lack of
concern for the importance of evaluating learner awareness towards the question
of the genre, as an integrative component of the reading skill (16 genre-oriented
128
questions vs. 59 non-genre oriented questions). This finding prompts an
investigation into the underlying principles which are likely to determine the
preference for non-genre oriented questions. This investigation will prove to be
particularly valuable as an attempt to answer Research Questions 1 and 3 (pp. 5-
6) presented in the introductory chapter of the present study.
Thus, in order to identify the underlying principles which seem to govern the
predominance of non-genre oriented questions in the coursebooks, an attempt will
be made to specify the types of non-genre oriented questions utilized by the
coursebook writers. The graph below summarizes the main findings.
As Graph 7 above suggests, it may be argued that the five coursebooks from the
corpus do not seem to regard the development of a genre-oriented approach to
reading as an essential element in equipping their audience with effective reading
20%
15%
21%
22%
14%
5%
3%
pre-reading scanning vocabulary recognition
post-reading inference retelling
grammar
20%
15%
21%
22%
14%
5%
3%
pre-reading scanning vocabulary recognition
post-reading inference retelling
grammar
(
Gra
p
h 7:
p
redominant non-
g
enre oriented
q
uestions in the coursebooks investi
g
ated
)
129
strategies. As it may be observed, the sources investigated present a diversity of
comprehension questions which do not seem to contemplate aspects of the genre-
text to which they are related. Moreover, even though Graph 7 displays a variety of
questions, they do not appear to have been equally distributed among the reading
sections. As a result, it is possible to observe a high percentage of post-reading
(22%) and pre-reading (20%) questions, for example. It may thus be argued that if,
on one hand, both types of question seem to be beneficial to learners, as far as
the development of reading strategies is concerned, on the other hand, the
reading of the target text is not a sine-qua-non requisite in order to perform those
tasks. Therefore, it may be claimed that such questions are not, in fact, checking
reading strategies. Edmundson (2005:37) avers that pre and post-reading
questions should not be placed under the label comprehension questions in the
sense that their corresponding answers are not found in the text. The same author
suggests that these types of questions should be placed in a separated section for
discussion.
A further salient feature revealed by Graph 7 is the high percentage of questions
aimed at vocabulary recognition (21%). Even though it has been argued that
vocabulary is likely to reveal aspects of the genre, the questions proposed for
learners in the sources of the corpus seem to reinforce the traditional view of
reading as the ability to identify and decode printed symbols. 21% of questions on
vocabulary appear to subscribe to the assumption that construction of meanings
tends to be associated with no more than the knowledge of lexical items. From this
perspective, texts are not contemplated as semantic units but, rather, as isolated
sentences grouped together.
130
Moreover, the high number of questions on scanning (15%) suggests that reading
for gist or identifying the communicative purpose of texts does not constitute a
priority in the sources investigated. Instead, target learners are expected to be
able to extract pieces of information spread throughout the text surface. These
preferences would seem to indicate a more traditional view of reading as the ability
to locate and copy information explicitly mentioned in the text.
However, the limited number of questions focusing on grammar (3%) may be
claimed as a positive shift in the sources investigated. It seems to suggest that
EFL learners at the intermediate level of language learning have been provided
with the opportunity to experiment with reading lessons rather than text-based
lessons (see subsection 2.1.2 on pp. 11 to 13 for the main distinction between the
two types of lessons). Most activities devised seemed to present grammar as a
tool in order to construct intended meanings within a given context. This finding
seems to suggest that although language has traditionally been analyzed and taught at
the level of the sentence, real language use seldom consists of sentences in isolation, but of
groups of sentences that form coherent texts (Thornbury, 1999:71)
The second half of Table 3.14 displays the number of genre-oriented questions in
the coursebooks analyzed. Firstly, it ought to be said that the results seem to
reveal a lack of concern for the importance of the concept of genre as an
integrative core in the development of EFL reading skills. Secondly, the
predominance of skimming-type questions (38% as displayed in Graph 8, p. 131)
gives rise to two questions which need discussing. One question is whether the
131
skimming questions were designed to verify a certain “genre awareness” of the
candidates, or whether they appear as no more than an incidental occurrence as
skimming in itself might be said to inherently contribute to the identification of the
genre. In short, it might be questioned whether the test designers intentionally
devised such questions as part of a concern for the generic features of the texts in
question. The other question is related to the degree to which the candidates to
the FCE are consciously applying their genre schemata when responding to these
questions. Due to the intuitive nature with which such questions seem to have
been elaborated, it would be supposed that the candidates are not indeed applying
their knowledge of the genre. The main finding regarding genre-oriented questions
are shown in Graph 8, below:
It also seems important to highlight certain conclusions which may be drawn from
the identification of the genre-oriented questions displayed in Graph 8. As may be
6%
31%
38%
25%
cohesion coherence skimming generic features
6%
31%
38%
25%
cohesion coherence skimming generic features
(
Gra
p
h 8:
p
redominant
g
enre-oriented
q
uestions in the coursebooks investi
g
ated
)
132
observed, 25% of the questions focused on generic features, 31% focused on
aspects of coherence and 6% contemplated cohesive ties. As already discussed,
the first group comprises questions focused on the identification of elements which
signal aspects of field, tenor and mode. These questions seem to have been
intentionally devised in order to raise learners’ awareness regarding genre
features. They have focused on two aspects of the genre-texts: a) naming the
genre-text itself (field) and b) identifying the source of the text in question as well
as the participants involved in the interaction (tenor). It may be argued that in order
to answer such questions, learners are expected to be familiar with a range of
texts and to resort to their genre-schemata when attempting to accomplish the
tasks proposed. The second group represents the majority of genre-oriented
questions/tasks. However, it may be argued that in the coursebooks analyzed the
development of genre awareness on the learners’ part would come as no more
than an incidental benefit. These tasks were merely restricted to ordering jumbled
paragraphs or numbering a list of events in the sequence in which they originally
occurred. None of the coursebooks from the corpus seem to have provided
learners with overt assistance as to what extent the discourse organizational
features (aspects of coherence) are likely to contribute to the identification of the
communicative purposes and, as a consequence, the identification of the genre in
question. The third group of questions, however, was restricted to no more than
the identification of elements of reference in the texts.
All in all, it is possible to infer that, to a certain extent, the variety of
comprehension question purposes, identified in the five coursebooks investigated,
appear to match with their main concern: equipping learners with a wide range of
133
reading strategies. However, a genre-oriented approach to texts is still an aim
worth fighting for, since it fails to be seen by the coursebook writers as an
integrating element capable of developing more effective reading strategies.
In the following subsection, conclusions will be drawn in the light of the
investigation of the four FCE reading papers from the corpus.
3.3.9.3. The FCE reading papers
Interestingly, the testing of reading in the four FCE reading papers analyzed also
focuses on non-genre oriented questions, as displayed in Table 3.15:
(Table 3.15: an overview of the comprehension questions identified in the FCE reading papers)
reading comprehension questions
¾
FCE reading paper À
Genre-oriented
Non-genre
oriented
June 2003 14 15
December 2003 14 22
June 2004 16 22
December 2004 15 23
Total 59 82
As may be observed in Table 3.15, above, most questions testing reading
comprehension in the FCE reading papers of the corpus are non-genre oriented,
with, specifically, a predominance of questions on inference and scanning. The
main findings regarding non-genre oriented questions are shown in Graph 9
below:
134
Most comprehension questions in the FCE reading papers examined seem to
focus on scanning (67%) i.e., identifying specific pieces of information on the text
surface. The questions on inference (33%), despite being extremely important for
the development of effective reading skills, do not reflect aspects specific to the
type of genre-text to which they are related.
As far as the genre-oriented questions are concerned, Graph 10 is an attempt to
summarize the main findings.
33%
67%
inference scanning
33%
67%
inference scanning
(
Gra
p
h 9:
p
redominant non-
g
enre oriented
q
uestions in the FCE readin
g
p
a
p
ers
)
52%
48%
skimming coherence
52%
48%
skimming coherence
(
Gra
p
h 10:
p
redominant
g
enre-oriented
q
uestions in the FCE readin
g
p
a
p
ers
)
135
As Graph 10 suggests, questions on skimming (52%) and coherence (48%) are
also predominant in the FCE reading papers examined. It may be claimed that test
designers seem to be mainly concerned with the identification of the
communicative purposes of the target text (field) and to what extent the schematic
organization/rhetorical schemata of the discourse is likely to promote the
identification of the communicative purposes of texts. The questions focusing on
skimming and coherence have been identified in Parts 1 and 3 of each test,
respectively.
136
CHAPTER 4
CONCLUSIONS
4.1. Final conclusions
As described in sub-section 3.3.1, the present study consisted of a three-staged
investigation. Firstly, there was an attempt to delineate the discourse community at
which the FCE Reading Paper is aimed (see sub-section 3.3.2 for discussion on
this topic). The ultimate aim at this initial stage was to either confirm or reject the
initial hypothesis whereby FCE candidates, as well as people engaged in
designing and administering the tests, could be regarded as a discourse
community. This part of the study was conducted in the light of Swales’ (1990)
concept of discourse community. Secondly, a quantitative-interpretative
description of the genre exemplars identified in both sources of the corpus, namely
coursebooks and FCE Reading Papers, was developed. This stage of the analysis
was conducted by considering salient layout features, communicative function(s),
source(s), writer and lexical and syntactic choices, among other variables which
may contribute for the identification of the global organization of the discourse.
Similarly, the second stage of the investigation attempted to focus on the
contributions that genre studies may provide for the development of more effective
reading lessons. Thirdly, the final stage of the investigation contemplated the
reading comprehension questions which accompanied the texts from both sources
of the corpus. The ultimate aim of the third stage was to determine to what extent
the questions proposed to learners/candidates were genre-oriented. The
subsequent paragraphs will focus on each of the stages previously outlined.
137
As far as the first stage of the analysis is concerned, the initial hypothesis was
confirmed. It was possible to conclude that there seems to exist a discourse
community which nurtures the FCE examination. This discourse community is
sustained by its members` set of shared common goals, among the other defining
characteristics postulated by Swales` (1990).
As highlighted above, the second stage of the analysis was particularly concerned
with a detailed quantitative-interpretative investigation of the genre-texts
encountered in the corpus. As far as the five coursebooks are concerned, the
investigation of the genre-texts presented in the 101 reading comprehension
sections appear to be directly related to research questions 1 and 2:
1) Do reading sections of coursebooks really reflect the demands of a globalized
society in which individuals are expected to experiment with texts as semantic
units rather than groups of isolated sentences?
2) To what extent can genre analysis contribute to the development of more
effective reading lessons and, as a result, guide learners towards becoming more
proficient readers and, potentially, as a consequence, improved EFL writers?
The analysis has revealed magazine/newspaper articles and stories as the two
predominant genres in the coursebooks investigated (see Graph 1, p. 96). The
investigation conducted in the light of Swales’ Working Definition of Genre (1990)
and Hoey’s genre-general macropatterns for text analysis seem to suggest that
the previously referred genres tend to be preferred by coursebook writers. As
discussed in Sub-section 3.3.5, this finding may be said to signal a tendency
among coursebooks in general and has led to the three following questions:
138
1. Does the predominance of newspaper/magazine articles reveal a tendency in
the global editorial market?
2.Does this predominance signal that newspaper/magazine articles constitute the
most popular genre among readers in general?
3.Is it possible to argue that the predominance of newspaper/magazine articles in
the coursebooks investigated reflect the predominance of the same genres in the
FCE Reading Paper ?
The first possible conclusion for such supremacy is that articles tend to be the
genre which is more likely to be encountered in most discourse communities all
over the world. Thus, this could be regarded as the genre which individuals are
more likely to encounter in real-life reading situations. In addition, the
predominance of articles may be interpreted as an attempt to engage learners in
reading texts which make real-life demands (question b, Sub-section 3.3.5, p.
111). Moreover, the process of identification of the predominant genres revealed
articles as an “umbrella genre” or a “primary genre”, i.e., a number of other texts
whose internal organization reflected a diversity of genres was presented under
the format of articles. This factor appears to corroborate the assumption that
articles consist of a genre of substantial relevance in the global editorial market
(Question a, Sub-section 3.3.5, p. 111). This situation may be explained through
the apparent growing relevance of either magazine/newspaper articles as a source
of information which is likely to reach the discourse communities from the most
remote geographical areas.
139
The findings previously described were confronted by the identification of the
salient genre-texts in the FCE reading papers of the corpus. As summarized in
Tables 3.10, 3.11, 3.12 and 3.13 (see Sub-section 3.3.6, p. 112 and 113),
magazine and/or newspaper articles have also been identified as a “primary
genre” in the FCE reading papers examined (see Sub-section 3.3.6, Question C,
p. 111). Again, even though a detailed investigation of the internal organization of
the genre-texts from both sources has revealed that a number of other genres has
been identified, a substantial number of texts were presented in the format of
articles which appears to highlight the supremacy of such genre within the global
editorial arena. As a result, there was an attempt to find evidence which would
allow to draw conclusions related to question d (see Sub-section 3.3.5., p. 111)
further transcribed.
d.Even though the coursebooks investigated do not constitute exemplars of exam
practice books, would it be possible to claim that, among other objectives, such
coursebooks are aimed at building up learners’ genre-schemata for the FCE
Reading Paper ?
Therefore, considering that articles constituted the major genre identified in both
sources, it may be inferred that even though the coursebooks investigated do not
constitute exemplars of exam practice books, they may be claimed to have been
aimed at building up learners’ genre-schemata. From such a perspective, it would
seem reasonable to argue that learners would benefit from such coursebooks as a
preparation tool for the FCE reading paper as far as the choice of genre-texts is
concerned. As a result, it could be inferred that learners are expected to feel
140
confident enough to take the FCE reading paper by the end of their three-year
language courses, at least as far as their genre-schemata are concerned.
In very general terms, it is also possible to argue that the procedures adopted in
order to identify the predominant genre-texts in both elements of the corpus (i.e.,
reading comprehension section + FCE reading papers) seem to suggest that
genre analysis may provide insightful contributions to more enlightened EFL
reading lessons (Research Question 2, Section 1.1). However, in order to achieve
this aim, it seems fundamental that teachers in general be aware of the underlying
principles which should nurture reading lessons in contrast to text-based lessons.
Moreover, by raising learners’ awareness regarding genre features, teachers may
also provide learners with opportunities to improve their writing abilities. This
finding suggests that a more comprehensive knowledge of how to achieve a given
communicative intent might lead learners to produce more readable texts. From
such a perspective, the positive Impact (see Sub-sections 2.2.5 and 2.2.6 for
discussion on this topic) to be experienced by learners, i.e., passing a given
proficiency test, would result as an incidental benefit.
However, the predominance of articles in both sources of the corpus appear to
signal a drawback in the three premises established by the FCE question
designers, as far as the variety and choice of target texts are concerned. As
already discussed (see Sub-section 3.3.3.1, p. 95), according to FCE questions
designers, reading papers should reflect the three premises further transcribed:
1) FCE candidates are expected to be able to read semi-authentic
texts of various kinds (informative and general interest ....
141
2) The texts are intended to cover a range of recently published
material and to appear authentic in form, presentation and content.
3) Pictures and other diagrams are used where appropriate to
illustrate the text (...). Some of the vocabulary may be simplified in
the texts to keep it within the FCE level but such changes are kept
to minimum.
As the exemplification of analysis illustrated through texts ONE and TWO (see pp.
98-106) seem to suggest, even though the reading comprehension sections from
each coursebook do not claim to relate specifically to the FCE reading paper, it is
possible to argue that, as far as the three premises are concerned, premises two
and three have apparently been met by the reading comprehension sections. The
texts from the coursebooks appeared authentic in form, presentation and content
(premise two). Similarly, a number of texts presented non-linear texts such as
illustrations and diagrams in order to illustrate them, as well as arouse learners’
topic schemata (Premise Three). However, as far as Premise One is concerned,
the percentages of occurrences of each genre-text identified in the coursebooks
has revealed that there seems to be a lack of variety among the sources, i.e., four
out of five coursebooks from the corpus did not meet the demands specified by the
FCE reading paper designers as far as the variety of genre is concerned.
In the third stage of the analysis the major aim was to focus on the investigation of
the reading comprehension questions from both sources of the corpus. There was
an attempt to determine to what extent the questions proposed were genre-
oriented i.e., to what extent they aimed at measuring learners’ and FCE
candidates’ awareness regarding genre features. The third stage of the analysis
142
seemed to be particularly related to Research Question 3 (see Section 1.1): how
genre-oriented are reading comprehension questions in the coursebooks and in
the FCE Reading Papers which constitute the corpus of analysis ?
The results obtained are believed to indicate whether coursebook writers and FCE
Reading Paper designers approach the question of genre as an integrating
element when assessing the development of reading skills. The reading
comprehension questions from both sources were divided into two major groups
according to their testing purposes: a) non-genre oriented and b) genre oriented
questions. The former comprised questions which focused on the testing of
grammatical knowledge, vocabulary recognition and identification of specific
information in the target text (scanning). The latter focused on the testing of lexical
and grammatical cohesion, identification of the overall message of the target text
(skimming/communicative purpose(s)), awareness of discourse organizational
features (aspects of coherence) and finally the knowledge of generic features (i.e.,
aspects of field, tenor and mode). Each testing purpose was illustrated with
sample questions from the corpus. The sample questions were selected due to the
fact that they seemed to constitute typical evidence of each testing purpose
previously outlined. The identification of the genre-oriented questions was based
on the assumption that the rationale of a given genre seems to govern the
rhetorical and patterned organization of a particular text. Genres influence register
variables related to aspects of field, tenor and mode and, as a consequence,
constrain the content of a particular communicative event.
143
The preliminary identification of the types of reading comprehension questions
proposed to learners/candidates generated the need to quantify the proportion of
genre-oriented questions in contrast to non-genre oriented questions. The ultimate
aim at this stage was to reveal the rationale which seemed to nurture the choice
and elaboration of reading comprehension questions in both sources. As displayed
in table 3.14 (see p. 127), most reading comprehension questions identified in the
coursebooks from the corpus were classified as non-genre oriented (59
occurrences in contrast to 16 occurrences). These figures seemed to suggest a
certain disregard on the part of coursebook writers regarding the question of
genre. Thus, the identification of aspects of genre tends to be ignored as an
integrative element which may contribute to the development of proficient reading.
As displayed in Graph 7 (see p. 128), most comprehension questions identified in
the coursebooks investigated consisted of pre and post reading questions,
vocabulary recognition, questions on inference, questions focused on grammatical
knowledge, scanning and retelling. Even though a variety of comprehension
questions was observed, most fail to acknowledge the aspects of the genre-text to
which they belonged. Moreover, the high percentage of questions focused on
vocabulary recognition (21%) seem to suggest that the underlying principle for the
elaboration of comprehension questions are limited to the notion of reading as the
decodification of printed symbols therefore not contemplating texts as semantic
units. This type of question appears to subscribe to the more traditional views
whereby FL proficient reading is acknowledged as homage to clarity and precision
(Silveira, 2002:92). From this perspective, negotiation and construction of
meanings are often overlooked. Furthermore, the high number of questions on
scanning (15%) appears to suggest that identifying the communicative purpose of
144
the target texts did not seem to constitute a source of concern among coursebook
writers in general. Rather, target learners were expected to be able to extract
pieces of information spread throughout the text surface. These preferences would
seem to indicate a view of reading as no more than the ability to locate and copy
information explicitly mentioned in the text. These questions, in turn, do not seem
to require extra-linguistic knowledge on the readers’ part.
As far as genre-oriented questions (i.e.: cohesion, coherence, skimming and
generic features) are concerned, the figures provided in the second half of Tables
3.14 and 3.15 seem to suggest that focusing on the concept of genre as an
essential element in the development of EFL reading skills did not seem a priority
or source of concern. The occurrences of explicit genre-oriented questions were
mainly incidental. The questions which apparently focused on generic aspects of
the target texts did not seem to have been intentionally or consciously elaborated.
Such apparent intuitive nature of the questions investigated makes it difficult to
determine whether the genre-oriented questions have been devised by
considering the relevance of a genuine genre-oriented approach to reading.
4.2. Potential pedagogical insights
The analysis of the predominant genres in both elements of the corpus appear to
suggest that texts constitute communicative events in the sense that they manifest
their communicative intent linguistically through written discourse. In addition, the
texts examined seem to constitute typical exemplars of their respective genres.
145
Thus, it is possible to conclude that the writer’s both lexical and syntactic choices
constitute fundamental elements. They seem to contribute to the internal/global
organization of texts, thus providing readers with improved chances of grasping
the message(s) being conveyed. In broader terms, it is possible to argue that the
analyses of the texts from the corpus in the light of Hoey’s (2001) macro-patterns
emerge as a precious opportunity to verify how genre-texts in general tend to be
organized and how writers manage to keep the flow of the discourse as well as the
readers’ attention throughout the reading process.
Moreover, pedagogically speaking, it may be claimed that each and every EFL
teacher should be aware of the potential benefits of providing learners with the
opportunity to inductively experiment with global macro-patterns for text
organization. This might, in turn, lead to more effective reading lessons and, as a
consequence, more proficient reading on the learners’ part. As Hoey (1997)
states, texts are interactive pieces of language and the analyses developed in the
present investigation seem to constitute a sample of what may be done in the
classroom as a means to praise more conscious reading: Since reading is about
getting meaning out of a text in interaction with that text and text analysis involves the
description of the interactive properties of a text with regard to its semantic structure,
there ought to be a close connection (Hoey, 1997:13). Furthermore, the investigation
of the reading comprehension questions provided a positive model for the types of
questions which should be exploited/elaborated in the EFL classroom. As Nuttall
(1996, pp. 32-34) claims, this view seems to suggest that the development of
effective reading may be claimed to make specific demands on both EFL teachers
and learners. Among such responsibilities, the same author highlights:
146
Teachers’ responsibilities
Encouraging learners to enjoy and value reading;
Having a thorough understanding of what effective reading comprises (i.e.:
language as a vehicle to convey meanings and textual organization);
Investigating learners’ potential so as to identify the skills they lack;
Selecting suitable material and devising effective tasks;
Equipping learners with the knowledge necessary to tackle the tasks and activities
successfully;
Ensuring learners are given equal opportunities to work productively according to their
own intellect.
Students’ responsibilities
Being actively engaged in the learning process;
Regarding reading as a source of enjoyment and personal development.
Monitoring their own comprehension: being aware of the processes they undergo in
order to understand the presuppositions spread within the text surface;
Establishing a dialogue with the text by taking risks: adopting and switching
strategies so as to achieve comprehension.
(Adapted from Nuttall, 1996:32-34)
Therefore, readers should be able, not only to construct texts, but also to
deconstruct them and identify which lexical and syntactic choices may enable the
writer to answer the set of questions that they have themselves established. The
interactive character of texts (i.e., the waltz between writer and reader) seems
undeniable and should be cultivated in the EFL classroom by encouraging
147
learners to employ the tools at their disposal in order to become fluent readers in
the target language.
In a nutshell, it seems that text analysis may well provide valuable contributions for
the teaching of EFL reading. Nevertheless, it seems of crucial importance that
both teachers and learners are aware of what effective reading lessons are
claimed to entail. Again, as Nuttall (1996, pp. 32-34) argues, teachers and learners
need to be aware of their responsibilities throughout the process.
Undertaking the process of investigating the genre-texts and the reading
comprehension sections from a genre-oriented perspective has been of great
value in terms of raising my own awareness towards what may be done in the EFL
classroom, as far as a genre-oriented approach to reading is concerned. The
pedagogical implications of this study appear to be related to the importance of
genre studies to the EFL teaching/learning scenario. Through the analysis
described in Chapter 3, I have concluded that it is possible to provide learners with
challenging opportunities to experiment with texts from a perspective which moves
beyond reading at the sentence level and acknowledges the communicative
nature of texts as the major feature which constrains format, lexis and register.
As discussed above, texts are samples of language in use. Therefore, learners
should be encouraged to view language as a resource, a means to an end. EFL
learners need to be aware that language consists of a powerful tool in order to
construct meanings. Therefore, it should be used in order to reflect the context in
which it is inserted, the interactants, their roles and aims. Learners need to be
148
encouraged to regard meaning as the starting point for the use of grammatical
resources and therefore use language more effectively as a means to express
their thoughts by considering the context and the co-text.
4.3. Limitations and further research
As discussed in the preceding section, the present study may have practical
pedagogical implications. However, it is worth examining its limitations. The
importance of the present discussion is justified by the fact that an attentive and
reflective look into the limitations of this study is likely to unveil relevant focal
points for future research in the area of genre studies.
In Chapters 1 and 2 the substantial relevance that coursebooks and proficiency
tests have gained within the English-teaching market was addressed. The former
have long been acknowledged as the major pedagogical resource in the language
classroom and their level of appropriateness and effectiveness have seldom been
questioned by learners, teachers and course administrators. The latter, by the
same token, seem to make specific demands on learners/candidates and have
been regarded as a means to achieve internationally recognized certification in an
increasingly competitive globalized world. Similarly, the need for preparation for
international examinations either during regular courses or during exam practices
courses, has placed considerable emphasis on the adoption of coursebooks
produced abroad.
149
However, if on one hand, the coursebooks produced abroad tend to be regarded
as reliable sources for learning and preparation, on the other hand, my experience
as an EFL teacher in a number of Brazilian private language schools, has
indicated that a significant percentage of EFL intermediate learners still tend to
feel insecure about undergoing proficiency tests. As far as reading is concerned, a
substantial number of learners tend to panic whenever confronted with lengthy
texts in the target language. As explained in Sub-section 1.1., the apparent lack of
self-confidence when tackling texts in the target language may spring from a
diversity of factors such as tight schedules, lack of expertise on teachers’ part and
Brazilian learners’ apparent lack of interest in reading in general. However, the
development of reading skills seems to be given a secondary role in the EFL
classroom when teachers often devote most of their class time to the development
of speaking and listening skills. It is possible to infer that the apparent lack of self-
confidence experienced by a number of Brazilian EFL learners as far as reading is
concerned might be nurtured by the traditional view of reading as a passive
activity. From this perspective, reading disregards the interaction between lower
and higher-level processing mechanisms. In fact, the precise identification of the
main causes which inhibit the development of effective reading skills among
Brazilian EFL learners did not constitute the focal point of the present
investigation. Further research in this area would be necessary in order to
delineate more precise conclusions on this issue. Thus, a detailed investigation of
how genre-oriented reading lessons may contribute as a tool for a rise in the level
of interest and, as a result, for more proficient reading emerges as a possibility.
From this perspective, not only coursebooks would be the focus but also teachers’
and learners’ attitudes towards reading. This same future research could involve
150
reading lessons observations, as well as interviews with teachers and learners.
Moreover, the investigation previously described could be developed in both
private language schools (i.e., with learners attending exam practice courses) and
schools from the public sector (i.e. with learners preparing for vestibular). At a
further stage, a contrastive analysis could be conducted in order to identify
differences and similarities among reading lessons in both environments.
Finally, it seems crucial to highlight that the present research study constitutes a
possible practical application of genre studies in the area of Applied Linguistics.
Thus, there is no attempt to provide universal status to the conclusions drawn. The
findings were based on the investigation of a restricted corpus. However, despite
the limited scope of this study, it may be argued that the results obtained may
function as a source of investigation into what has occurred in the reading
classroom.
BIBLIOGRAPHY
ALDERSON, J. C., & HUGHES, A. (eds). 1981. Issues in Language Testing. London:
The British Council.
ALDERSON, J. C., URQUHART, A. H. (Ed.) 1984. Reading in a Foreign Language.
London: Longman.
BAKHTIN, M. & VOLOCHINOV V. 1929. Marxismo e filosofia da linguagem. 7. ed.
São Paulo: Husitec.
BARNET, M. A. 1989. More than meets the eye. Foreign Language reading: theory
and practice. Englewook Cliffs, New Jersey: Prentice.
BACKMAN, L. F. & PALMER, A. S. 1997. Language testing in Practice. Oxford:
Oxford University Press.
BHATIA, V. 1993. Analysing Genre: Language Use in Professional Settings.
London: Longman
BOLITHO, R. 2004. Global coursebooks, local contexts. In Approach. Havana, Cuba:
British Council. Pp. 7-11
Cambridge First Certificate in English. 2001. UCLES.
CARRELL, P. L. 1987. Readability in ESL. Reading in a Foreign Language. 4/1: 21-
40.
CELCE – MURCIA, M., OLSHSTAIN, E. 2000. Reading. In Discourse & context in
English language teaching. Chapter on Reading. Cambridge: Cambridge
University Press.
COLLINS, A., BROWN, J., LARKIN, K. 1979. Inference in Text Understanding. In:
New Directions in Discourse Processing. Freedle, R. O., Collins, A. C. (Eds.)
New York: Ablex Publishing. Pp. 385-407
CORACINI, M. J. 2002. A aula de leitura: um jogo de ilusões. In: O Jogo discursivo
na aula de leitura: língua materna e língua estrangeira. Campinas: Pontes. pp.
27-33.
CRYSTAL, D. 1997. English as a global language. Cambridge: Cambridge
University Press.
CUNNINGSWORTH, A. 1989. Evaluating and Selecting EFL Teaching Materials.
London: Heinemann
DE BEAUGRANDE, R. and DRESSLER. W. 1981. Introduction to Text Linguistics.
New York: Longman.
DEVITT, A. J. 2004. Writing Genres. Carbondale: Southern Illinois University Press.
DROGA, L., HUMPHREY 2002. Getting started with functional grammar. Berry,
NSW: Target Texts.
EDMUNDSON, M. V. A. 2004. Leitura e compreensão de textos no livro didático de
língua Inglesa. Unpublished. Dissertação de Mestrado: UFPE, 2002.
--------------------, M. V. A. 2005. Analysing Types of Questions for Reading
Comprehension Texts. In Approach: British Council. Pp 32-37.
FAIRCLOUGH, N. 1989. Language and Power. London: Longman.
FCE First Certificate in English Handbook. English as a Foreign Language. 2001.
UCLES.
GAZOTTI, M. A. 1999. Genres: an alternative to ELT. In New Routes in ELT. São
Paulo: Disal.
GOODMAN, K.S., GOODMAN, Y.M. 1977. Learning about Psychological Processes
by Analyzing Oral Reading. Harvard Educational Review, Vol. 47. No. 3. pp.
317-333.
GRADDOL, D. 1999. The decline of the native speaker. In English in a changing
world – L’anglais dans un monde changeant. Oxford. AILA (International
Association of Applied Linguistics) Review 13.
GRADDOL, D. 2006. English Next. London: British Council.
GRELLET, F. 1981. Developing reading skills: a practical guide to reading
comprehension exercises. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
--------------, F. 1995. Developing reading skills. Cambridge: Cambridge University
Press.
GREEN, T. & HAWKEY, R. 2004. Test Washback and Impact: What do they mean
and why do they matter?. In MET Vol 13, no 4. Pp 66-70.
HALLIDAY, M.A.K &HASAN, R. 1976. Cohesion in English. London: Longman.
HASAN, R. 1989. The identity of a text. In: HALLIDAY, M.A.K & HASAN, R.
Language, context and text: Aspects of language in a social-semiotic
perspective. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
HOEY, M. P. 1986. Overlapping patterns of discourse organization and their
implications for clause relational analysis of problem-solution texts. In COOPER,
C & GREENBAUN , S. (eds) Annual Review of Written Discourse and Writing.
London: Sage.
HOEY, M. P. 1995. A Tentative Map of Discourse Study and their Place in
Linguistics. In Ilha do Desterro, Vol 9. Florianópolis: UFSC. Pp 9-27.
HOEY, M. P. 1997. How can text analysis help us to teach reading?. Inédito. Plenary.
XXVI IATEFL Conference. Brighton: England.
-----------------. 2001. Textual Interaction. Routledge: London.
KLEIMAN, A. 2001. Leitura: ensino e pesquisa. Campinas: Pontes.
----------------,2002. Oficina de leitura: teoria e prática. Campinas: Pontes.
KOCH, I. V. 2003. A coesão textual. São Paulo: Editora Contexto.
LOPES, J. C. 1999. A genre analysis of tests of English in University Entrance
Examinations. Unpublished MA dissertation. Niterói: UFF.
MALINOWSKI, B. 1926. The problem of meaning in primitive languages. Supplement
to OGDEN, C.K., RICHARDS, I.A. “Meaning of Meaning”. London: Kegan Paul.
Cited by CRIPER, C., WIDDOWSON, H.G. 1975. In ALLEN & CODER. 1975.
pp. 155-210.
MARCUSCHI, L. A. 2002. Compreensão de textos: algumas reflexões. In O livro
didático de português: algumas reflexões. Rio de Janeiro: Editora Lucerna.
MARCUSCHI, L. A. 2002. Gêneros textuais: definição e funcionalidade. In Gêneros
Textuais e Ensino. Rio de Janeiro: Editora Lucerna.
MOTTA-ROTH. 1995. Rhetorical Features and Disciplinary Cultures: a genre-based
study of academic book reviews in Linguistics, Chemistry and Economy.
Unpublished Doctoral thesis. Florianópolis: UFSC.
NUTTALL, C. 1996. Teaching Reading Skills in a Foreign Language. London:
Heinenmann.
PALTRIDGE, B. 1996. Genre, text type, and the language classroom. ELT Journal
Volume 50 No 3. Oxford University Press, pp. 231-243.
RICHARDS, J. C. 2005. What’ new in the Teaching of Reading?. In
Approach.Havana, Cuba: British Council. Pp. 10-13
SANTOS, F. 2004. O livro didático na perspective do ensino de leitura em LE.
Unpublished MA dissertation. Niterói: UFF.
SHEPHERD, D. 1984. A natureza da linguagem escrita em contraste com a
linguagem falada. In Letras, Curitiba (33) 145-162. UFPR.
SILVA, M. C. 2004.A avaliação da leitura em língua estrangeira explorando gêneros
textuais. Unpublished Doctoral Thesis. Niterói: UFF.
SILVEIRA, M. E. K. 2002. Conhecimento lexical e leitura em língua estrangeira: no limite do
imponderável. Rio de Janeiro: Cadernos de Letras da UFF nº 24.
STEVENS, K. 1980. The effect of Background knowledge on the reading
comprehension of ninth grades. In Journal of Reading Behavior. Vol. XII. No. 2.
Pp. 151-154.
SWALES, J. M. 1990. Genre analysis: English in academic and research settings.
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
THORNBURY, S. 1999. How to teach grammar. London: Longman.
THORNDIKE, E.L. Adult Learning. New York: MacMillan. Cited in CIARK, R. “Adult
Theories, Child Strategies”. In ALLEN, J.P.B., CORDER, S.P. (1975) (Eds.) The
Edinburgh Course in Applied Linguistics. Volume 2. Oxford: Oxford University
Press. Pp. 291-347.
TOMLINSON, B. 2005. Testing to learn: a personal view of language testing. In ELT
Journal Volume 59. Oxford University Press. Pp 39-46.
TRIBBLE, C. 1996. Writing. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Tasks 32-38.
WALLACE, C. 1992. Reading. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
APPENDICES
APPENDIX 1
Sample texts from the five coursebooks
Coursebook “Alfa”
Text 1: Flat-sharing – the inside story.
Text 2: Eat as much as you like – and stay healthy!
Text 3: Nightmare journeys
Text 4: Sun Tours: short breaks in New York
APPENDIX 1
Sample texts from the five coursebooks
Coursebook “Beta”
Text 1: Strategies for keeping your job.
Text 2: Culture check
Text 3: Learning styles
Text 4: The blue lights of Silver Cliff
APPENDIX 1
Sample texts from the five coursebooks
Coursebook “Gama”
Text 1: “Reading and discussion”.
Text 2: “The Randolf Hotel”.
Text 3: “Reading: Read the five parts of the following story.
See if you notice anything unusual’.
Text 4: Are they crazy or am I?
APPENDIX 1
Sample texts from the five coursebooks
Coursebook “Delta”
Text 1: Words fail me.
Text 2: Excuse me, is it day or night ?
Text 3: Caring students watch over teacher.
Text 4: My day as a 75-year-old.
APPENDIX 1
Sample texts from the five coursebooks
Coursebook “Épsilon
Text 1: Fictional heroes never die.
Text 2: “Reading: lesson 18.”
Text 3: “Reading: lesson 31.”
Text 4: Lost in the Pacific.
APPENDIX 2
The four FCE reading papers
FCE June/2003
FCE December/2003
FCE June/2004
FCE December/2004
APPENDIX 3
University of
Cambridge
Researchers and
exam designers
Examiners
Teachers
Preparatory
schools and
examination
boards
candidates
UCLES
Ï Ï Ï Ï Ï Ï
EFL
Ï Ï Ï Ï Ï Ï
TEFL
Ï Ï Ï Ï Ï
-
ALTE
Ï Ï Ï Ï Ï
-
CPE
Ï Ï Ï Ï Ï Ï
CAE
Ï Ï Ï Ï Ï Ï
FCE
Ï Ï Ï Ï Ï Ï
PET
Ï Ï Ï Ï Ï Ï
KET
Ï Ï Ï Ï Ï Ï
3.1.: Sam
p
les of s
p
ecific acron
y
ms
(Ï) This symbol indicates that the discourse community members are
expected to be familiar with this particular acronym.
( - ) This symbol indicates that the discourse community members are
not expected to be familiar with this particular acronym.
APPENDIX 4
Specific lexis
University of
Cambridge
Researchers and
exam designers
Examiners
Teachers
Preparatory
schools and
examination
boards
candidates
Results
Ï Ï Ï Ï Ï Ï
skills
-
Ï Ï Ï
-
Ï
Communicative strategies
-
Ï Ï Ï
-
Ï
Spelling
-
Ï Ï Ï
-
Ï
consistency
-
Ï Ï Ï
- -
Subsidiary points
-
Ï
Ï Ï
- -
Proficiency
Ï Ï Ï Ï Ï Ï
Question paper
Ï Ï Ï Ï Ï Ï
Overseas teaching situations
Ï Ï Ï Ï
-
Ï
Grading
Ï Ï Ï Ï
- -
Monitoring procedures -
Ï
-
Ï
- -
Handbook
Ï Ï Ï Ï
- -
Sample material
Ï Ï Ï Ï
- -
Editing -
Ï
- - - -
Question paper construction
Ï Ï
- - - -
Item analysis -
Ï Ï Ï
-
Revision -
Ï Ï Ï
-
Ï
Examination reports
Ï Ï Ï Ï
- -
Code of Practice
Ï Ï Ï
- - -
Reporting of test results
Ï Ï Ï Ï
- -
Empirical research
Ï Ï
- - - -
Score
Ï Ï Ï Ï
-
Ï
Topics -
Ï Ï Ï
-
Ï
guidelines
Ï Ï Ï Ï
-
Listening/speaking/writing/reading
test
-
Ï Ï Ï Ï Ï
Use of English
Ï Ï Ï Ï
-
Ï
Multiple-choice cloze, open cloze -
Ï Ï Ï
-
Ï
Framework -
Ï Ï Ï
- -
Malpractice committee
Ï Ï Ï
- - -
Supplementing material -
Ï Ï Ï
- -
Candidate answer sheet -
Ï Ï Ï
- -
Feedback
Ï Ï Ï Ï
- -
Task-specific mark scheme -
Ï Ï Ï
- -
Accuracy -
Ï Ï Ï
-
Ï
Seminars
Ï Ï Ï Ï
-
Ï
Interlocutor/assessor frames -
Ï Ï Ï
- -
Mark allocation -
Ï Ï Ï
- -
Tapescript -
Ï Ï Ï
-
Ï
Certificates
Ï Ï Ï Ï Ï Ï
Applied Linguistics
Ï Ï Ï Ï
- -
3.2.: Sam
p
les of s
p
ecific lexis
APPENDIX 5
(Table 3.3.: genre-texts identified in the coursebooks)
1
Coursebooks
¾
Genre-texts
À
Α
(Alfa)
Β
(Beta)
Γ
(Gama)
Δ
(Delta)
Ε
(Épsilon)
occurrences
of the
genre-texts
Articles Magazine
or
Newspaper
8
11 3 11 13 46
Biographical sketch
1 1 4 6
Brochure Travel
1 1 2
Classified ads Job
opportunities
1 1
Conversations
3 3
Diary entry
1 1
Graphs
1 1
Interviews
1 1 2
Invitations
1 1
Personal
3 3
Letters
Advice
(asking
for/giving)
1 2 2 5
Narrative/story
5 2 3 1 2 13
Questionnaire/quiz
1 2 3
Personal accounts
2 4 3 9
Poems
1 1
Postcards
1 1
Book
1 1 2
Reviews
Place
1 1
Total number of reading sections
20 16 27 12 26
1
The categories presented in Graphs 1 to 6 have been labeled according to the names they receive in
western countries:
* Articles = mainly newspaper and magazine articles covering a wide range of topics such as science,
entertainment, politics etc.
* Stories = fictional or non-fictional narratives, biographical sketches, anecdotes etc.
* Personal accounts = texts in which the writers describes his/her own experience
* Miscellaneous - term created by the author in order to address the genres which had a single
occurrence in the corpus examined (e.g.: ads, diary entries, graphs, invitations, poems and
postcards)
APPENDIX 6
Sample Text ONE
Do we really need to sleep?
APPENDIX 7
Sample Text TWO
Friends again forever.
APPENDIX 8
Sample Text
Biographical sketch
APPENDIX 9
Sample Text
Travel brochure
APPENDIX 10
Sample Text
An interview
Livros Grátis
( http://www.livrosgratis.com.br )
Milhares de Livros para Download:
Baixar livros de Administração
Baixar livros de Agronomia
Baixar livros de Arquitetura
Baixar livros de Artes
Baixar livros de Astronomia
Baixar livros de Biologia Geral
Baixar livros de Ciência da Computação
Baixar livros de Ciência da Informação
Baixar livros de Ciência Política
Baixar livros de Ciências da Saúde
Baixar livros de Comunicação
Baixar livros do Conselho Nacional de Educação - CNE
Baixar livros de Defesa civil
Baixar livros de Direito
Baixar livros de Direitos humanos
Baixar livros de Economia
Baixar livros de Economia Doméstica
Baixar livros de Educação
Baixar livros de Educação - Trânsito
Baixar livros de Educação Física
Baixar livros de Engenharia Aeroespacial
Baixar livros de Farmácia
Baixar livros de Filosofia
Baixar livros de Física
Baixar livros de Geociências
Baixar livros de Geografia
Baixar livros de História
Baixar livros de Línguas
Baixar livros de Literatura
Baixar livros de Literatura de Cordel
Baixar livros de Literatura Infantil
Baixar livros de Matemática
Baixar livros de Medicina
Baixar livros de Medicina Veterinária
Baixar livros de Meio Ambiente
Baixar livros de Meteorologia
Baixar Monografias e TCC
Baixar livros Multidisciplinar
Baixar livros de Música
Baixar livros de Psicologia
Baixar livros de Química
Baixar livros de Saúde Coletiva
Baixar livros de Serviço Social
Baixar livros de Sociologia
Baixar livros de Teologia
Baixar livros de Trabalho
Baixar livros de Turismo